Zum Hauptinhalt springen

The contribution of texture contrasts and combinations to food acceptance across cultures.

Pellegrino, R ; Cheon, BK ; et al.
In: Journal of texture studies, Jg. 51 (2020-04-01), Heft 2, S. 225-231
Online academicJournal

The contribution of texture contrasts and combinations to food acceptance across cultures 

Texture has long been considered an important attribute for food acceptance. However, which specific textural characteristics contribute to overall acceptance of a food is not well understood. It has been suggested that texture contrasts and combinations are a universal feature in giving foods a desirable texture, yet this notion is largely based upon anecdotal data. This study uses multiple survey research methods to assess the importance of texture contrast and combinations across cultures (Poland, U.S.A., and Singapore). Participants (N = 288) completed a survey that included overt measures of food texture contrast importance as well as free response questions regarding texture. The overall importance of texture for food liking was not different across the populations. However, the participants from Singapore and Poland gave more importance to a desirable food having multiple textures than the U.S.A. cohort. When looking at free responses, participants were twice as likely to mention combinations (multiple textures) with a texture contrast when describing foods they liked, in comparison to foods they disliked. This was observed across all three cultures. However, the type and quantity of texture terms used within combinations were different among cultures. For instance, Singaporeans enjoyed more texturally diverse food combinations than the other two cultures. These findings highlight the importance of texture contrasts and combinations in three distinct cultures.

Keywords: combinations; contrasts; cross cultural; food preferences

INTRODUCTION

Texture is the material property that arises from the combination of structural, mechanical, and surface properties of foods which are detected through the senses of vision, hearing, and touch. In other words, texture is the way food and drink feels in one's mouth. Textures play an important role in food acceptance, with a majority of individuals giving it high importance in comparison to other characteristics of food (Luckett & Seo, [10]; Szczesniak, [15]; Szczesniak & Kahn, [18]). The importance of texture to palatability has been reported to be universal across populations (Rohm, [12]). Additionally, texture has been shown to be more important than flavor in the rejection of some foods for adults and children alike (Cardello, [2]; Szczesniak, [16]).

Texture perception often begins with the eyes, but is centered on tactile sensations from within the oral cavity. The movement of the jaw and tongue, along with lubrication from saliva, manipulate the texture to make it safe for ingestion. Throughout the manipulation step, the texture of the food changes dynamically. Nomenclature for describing textural qualities was first developed in the 1960s (Szczesniak, [14]). The terms classified were designed to be simple in wording to relate fundamental, measurable properties with perceptual experience. Specifically, texture terms were classified into three main classes of characteristics and subsequent primary or secondary properties (see Table). Three main classes of characteristics were defined: mechanical, geometrical and other. Mechanical characteristics included texture properties that relate to applied stress (e.g., hardness, elasticity). Geometrical characteristics have properties related to structure and appearance (e.g., particle size and shape) while other characteristics included mouthfeel properties that do not fall into the first two categories (e.g., moisture and fat content). However, as mentioned by Szczesniak and later Guinard and Mazzucchelli ([5]), some terms are more complex and may fit across categories or be context‐specific like juiciness (Guinard & Mazzucchelli, [5]). Studies on consumer attitudes toward food attributes show that texture terms, such as crisp/crispy, crunch/crunchy, creamy and juicy, are very commonly reported by consumers (Luckett & Seo, [10]; Szczesniak & Kahn, [18]). Furthermore, in similar cross‐cultural studies, overlap and differences in texture terms frequently used for foods among several populations have been shown (Lawless, Vanne, & Tuorila, [9]; Nishinari et al., [11]; Rohm, [12]). A list for 54 English texture terms for foods and their equivalent term in 22 languages has also been compiled (Drake, [4]). Culturally specific foods and styles of cooking change the types of textures experienced and may emphasize or deemphasize texture attributes. For instance, many Asian dishes incorporate cooked cartilage which is primarily a texturized food that lacks flavor. This may help explain why the Japanese have over 400 texture terms (Hayakawa et al., [7]; Yoshikawa, Nishimaru, Tashiro, & Yoshida, [20]). As mentioned, most cross‐cultural research in texture has examined types of terms and their salience among populations while ignoring texture combinations present within a food.

Classification of textural characteristics by three main classes (mechanical, geometric, and other)

PrimarySecondaryExamples
Mechanical characteristics
HardnessSoft, firm, hard
CohesivenessBrittlenessCrumbly, crunchy, brittle
ChewinessTender, chewy, tough
GumminessShort, mealy, pasty, gummy
ViscosityThin, viscous
ElasticityPlastic, elastic
AdhesivenessSticky, tacky, gooey
Geometric characteristics
Particle size and shapeGritty, grainy, course
Particle shape and orientationFibrous, cellular, crystalline
Other characteristics
Moisture contentDry, moist, wet, watery
Fat contentOilinessOily
GreasinessGreasy

1 Adapted from Szczesniak ([14]) by Guinard and Mazzucchelli ([5]).

There are several ways in which a food can contain multiple textures, many of which are not mutually exclusive. In order to better understand a multitextured food, this study looks to further classify multiple textured foods based upon previous descriptions. In the simplest form, a food can statically possess multiple textures (i.e., chewy cheese and the crispy crust of pizza). As mentioned previously, foods can possess different texture types which manifest temporally during the eating process. So, a food which contains a singular texture in the static state can be perceived to have multiple textures dynamically, for example chocolate moving from a hard texture upon the first bite to a smooth texture prior to swallowing. A change in texture and other flavor properties has been discussed as a key for high palatability of food (Hyde & Witherly, [8]). Different textures may also be classified depending on the energy required to process or manipulate them during consumption, and texture changes may move between two similar energy requirements or one having higher or lower (contrasting) energy needs than the other (Szczesniak & Kahn, [13]). For instance, when biting into a baguette, one may perceive contrasting textures from the hard exterior to the soft interior, or a high energy to low energy texture change. However, after bolus formation one may perceive both soft and moist textures concurrently, both of which require a low level of oral processing energy. Foods that are reported to have texture characteristics that require contrasting energy needs to process orally are deemed to have a texture contrast. Conversely, a food with multiple textures of the same oral processing energy requirements only contains a texture combination. More plainly a contrast always involves a texture combination, but a texture combination does not always imply a contrast is present.

The current study was designed to look at cross‐cultural differences of liked and disliked textures among three different populations: (a) North American (U.S.A, N = 124), (b) European (Poland, N = 73), and Asian (Singapore, N = 91). The survey sites were chosen to provide a broad perspective towards texture contrasts across three highly populated continents. Individuals from each population were asked to rate the importance of texture and foods having multiple textures. They were then asked to describe textures for their most liked and disliked foods. An online survey was used to collect responses and a coding schema based on an established texture classification system was used to analyze results. Building on previous work, we anticipate universal liking for texture and multiple textures within food; however, cultural shifts in types of textures mentioned for liked and disliked foods are expected.

METHODS AND MATERIALS

Participants

A total of 288 individuals with an age ranging from 18 to 77 years old (mean [M] = 30.24, SD = 11.91) participated in the study. Participants in the U.S.A. and Poland were recruited from the general population, while Singaporean participants were recruited from a student population. Thus, data was gathered from populations on three separate continents, Europe, Asia, and North America (see Table).

Descriptive statistics for age, gender, height, and weight of each sample by country

SexCountryNMinMaxMeanSD
MenAll menAge12518.070.028.510.4
BMI17.346.923.95.0
PolandAge5018.057.028.28.2
BMI17.338.321.93.7
SingaporeAge4021.030.023.81.9
BMI18.630.022.72.5
U.S.A.Age3519.070.034.315.2
BMI17.846.928.36.0
WomanAll womenAge16318.077.031.412.7
BMI18.246.824.76.8
PolandAge2318.052.028.48.4
BMI18.731.625.33.4
SingaporeAge5121.026.021.81.1
BMI12.326.720.42.6
U.S.A.Age8920.077.037.713.4
BMI18.246.827.07.9

Survey

A separate online survey was deployed for each participating population. The survey was presented in English for Singapore and U.S.A., while the Polish authors translated and presented their questions in Polish. The survey began with a general definition for texture (adapted from Szczesniak ([17]))

Texture is the sensation from the structural, mechanical, and surface properties of the foods detected through the senses of vision, hearing, and touch. In other words, texture is the way food and drink feels in your mouth.

Next, participants were asked to use a 7‐point category scale to assess (a) texture's importance to food liking ("Not Important at All" to "Extremely Important") and (b) level of agreement to the enjoyment of multiple textures ("Extremely Disagree" to" Extremely Agree").

  • In general, how important is texture to your liking of a food?
  • In order for a food to be enjoyable, it has to have multiple textures.

After ratings, the participants were asked to specify their most liked and disliked food or dish. They were then asked to list three texture qualities that make them like or dislike the food or dish. The supplemental text boxes were not mandatory in case the food/dish did not have three liked or disliked texture characteristics. Additionally, the participants were asked for their gender, age, nationality, native language, height, and weight.

Statistical analysis

Only individuals claiming nationality that matched the corresponding population of interest was analyzed. Analysis was completed in SAS 9.4 (SAS Institute, Inc., Cary, NC). The rating of texture importance and level of agreement to the enjoyment of multiple textures were analyzed through a one‐way ANCOVA with population as the predictive factor. Age and BMI were used as cofactors in these models.

The frequency of common liked and disliked texture terms were quantified for each population and the frequency of those popular terms (>5 mentions) was compared. A Fisher's exact test was used and post hoc comparisons among populations were Bonferroni corrected. Significant terms that may be due to language (especially Polish) were verified by a native Polish speaking author.

Texture quality terms were categorized into an established classification system for texture types (Guinard & Mazzucchelli, [5]; Szczesniak, [14]) (Table). Specifically, the class of texture characteristic (mechanical, geometric and other) and the subsequent primary and secondary characteristics were recorded in separate columns for each texture term. Next, three texture variables were calculated from the coded categories: texture contrast, texture classes, and texture combinations. A texture contrast was defined as a food being described by an active and passive texture (e.g., hard/crispy and soft/moist). A passive texture needs a low amount of energy to process the food (moist, soft) while an active texture needs a high amount of energy to process (Szczesniak & Kahn, [13]). Texture contrast was a binary variable (0 or 1)—either there was or there was not a contrast between two or more textures for a particular food. Texture combinations represented the number of different primary or secondary characteristics for a particular food. Texture classes represented the number of different classes of texture characteristics (mechanical, geometric and other), providing a measure of the diversity in texture terms used. Texture combinations and classes were continuous variables with a range from 0 to 3. Texture contrasts and combinations have been described in previous literature (Szczesniak & Kahn, [13]). For texture combination and class variables, a mixed effects model was used in which liked and disliked terms were used as the within variables and population as the between participants variable. A binomial mixed regression model was used to measure contrast differences within liked foods and among populations. For all three models, age and BMI were set as covariates.

RESULTS

There were no differences in the importance of texture to food liking among populations (F2, 269 = 1.62, p =.20, d = 0.17), with each rating food texture relatively important for liking: U.S.A. (Mean ± SD = 5.14 ± 1.51), Poland (5.12 ± 1.31), and Singapore (5.35 ± 1.25). However, the U.S.A. population (3.42 ± 1.53) put less importance on foods having multiple textures in order to be enjoyed compared to Polish (4.26 ± 1.75) and Singapore (4.39 ± 1.32) populations (F2, 273 = 10.12, p < .001, d = 0.53).

Differences among popular terms (>5 mentions) across populations within liking are marked in Table (for a list frequency for all terms, see Table S1. Soft was widely listed when describing liked foods and was either the first or second most popular descriptor for liked foods across all populations. Smooth, crispy, and crunchy were also commonly used to describe liked foods. Across populations there was less similarity in the terms to describe disliked foods.

List of popular texture terms used for liked and disliked foods

Liked foodsDisliked foods
TermsPolishSingaporeU.S.A.TermsPolishSingaporeU.S.A.
Chewy1.88 b9.35 a7.24 aChewy6.854.004.10
Creamy1.88 b3.27 ab7.24 aCrunchy0 b5.71 a1.87 ab
Crispy13.757.018.62Dry4.793.432.61
Crunchy0.63 c7.01 b18.62 aFatty4.11 a0 b0.37 b
Elastic4.38 a0 b0 bGrainy0.680.574.10
Firm5.63 a0.47 b4.14 aGritty0 b0 b3.73 a
Hard0.001.872.41Hard0 b13.14 a1.87 b
Juicy1.253.272.07Lumpy2.741.141.87
Layered3.13 a0 b0.34 abMushy2.74 a8 ab13.06 a
Melty2.500.932.41Rough0.68 b8 a1.49 b
Moist4.383.272.41Rubbery2.741.141.87
Rough0.63 ab4.67 a0.69 bSlimy0 b9.14 a12.31 a
Smooth5.63 b16.36 a9.66 abSlippery5.481.141.49
Soft18.7519.1613.10Soft4.798.573.73
Springy4.38 a0.47 ab0 bSquishy2.742.863.73
Tender6.885.145.52Sticky10.27 a4 ab2.24 b
Stringy6.85 a0 b2.99 ab
Tough10.273.434.48
Watery4.792.861.87

2 Note : All values are expressed as a percentage of total texture responses. Terms in the same row that do not share a letter are significantly different from each other (p  > .05).

A visualization of popular texture terms for liked and disliked foods are shown in Figure. Populations used different popular terms for liked and disliked foods (χ2 = 154.10, p < .001 and χ2 = 204.733, p < .001, respectively) foods. As shown in Figure , more texture combinations and texture contrasts were mentioned for liked foods compared to disliked foods (F1, 271 = 12.73, p < .001, d = 0.26 and F1, 419 = 63.01, p < .001, OR = 2.68, d = 0.55, respectively), and this was independent of culture (F2, 269 = 2.05, p =.13 and F2, 419 = 2.56, p =.08, respectively). However, an interaction between population and food liking was observed for texture characteristics (F2, 271 = 7.56, p < .001, d = 0.38; Figure). The Singapore population mentioned more texture characteristics for liked compared to disliked foods while the opposite trend was shown for U.S.A. and Polish populations. There were no other significant effects (p > .05).

jtxs12494-fig-0001.jpg

jtxs12494-fig-0002.jpg

jtxs12494-fig-0003.jpg

DISCUSSION

The classification of texture may fall under its main class of characteristics and subsequent primary and secondary properties as defined by Szczesniak ([14]). In this study, we examined these notions and defining mechanisms across populations to consider the consistency and cultural‐specificity of consumer attitudes towards texture contrasts and combinations across three distinct cultures.

As expected, texture in foods is important with all populations in the study reporting similarly high levels of importance. Texture is universally recognized as integral to making foods enjoyable to eat. Two similarly designed surveys asking individuals to openly report food attributes that contribute to the liking of several (30+) types of foods showed texture as the most frequently mentioned attribute followed by flavor (Luckett & Seo, [10]; Szczesniak & Kahn, [18]). Given the dynamic nature of texture changes during consumption, individual texture attributes are often not perceived in isolation, but accompanied by other textures. The importance of multiple textures to food appreciation was confirmed across the populations in the current study. In particular, the North American (U.S.A.) population rated the importance for foods having multiple textures to be less important for food pleasantness than the other two populations, even when controlling for age and BMI.

The presence of multiple textures happens during initial evaluation and during mastication as the texture typically shifts from high to low energy. Thus, at any stage, there may be different combinations of textures and they may or may not be contrasting. We show that all individuals, irrelevant of culture, report more combinations and contrasts in liked foods/dishes compared to disliked foods/dishes. Therefore, although the U.S.A. population were found to place less importance on foods having multiple textures, these textures were still present among the most palatable foods. Similarly, Szczesniak and Kahn ([13]) were the first to point out the importance of texture contrasts and combinations for a US population stating the "polarity between stimulating/energetic (i.e., firm, crisp) and passive/soothing (i.e., soft, creamy) texture characteristics form the foundation of many highly desirable texture combinations" (Szczesniak & Kahn, [13]). In this paper, they describe four scenarios incorporating texture combinations and contrasts to increase enjoyment: (a) within a meal, (b) on the plate, (c) within a multiphase food, and (d) within a uniphase food during consumption. Whereas within meal texture combinations may demonstrate low to high energy shifts in texture (e.g., soup to meat), most answers to our survey dealt with contrasts of varying combinations for the multiphase or uniphase foods (e.g., steak being crispy then tender or smooth).

The textures within combinations and contrasts were not the same across cultures with many different texture terms being reported for liked and disliked foods (Figure). Past studies have examined texture term usage across populations with differences being demonstrated between and within cultures, consumers, and experts (Blancher et al., [1]; Cardello et al., [3]; Drake, [4]; Lawless et al., [9]; Nishinari et al., [11]). For instance, it has been shown that English speakers use far less texture terms (~70) (Drake, [4]; Szczesniak & Kleyn, [19]) than those from Japan (~400) (Hayakawa et al., [7]; Yoshikawa et al., [20]), as the Japanese language includes many more onomatopoeic words for descriptions (e.g., fizz, crackle) (Hayakawa, Hatae, & Shimada, [6]). In our study, we focused on collecting an unrestrained consumer lexicon for liked and dislike foods. Interestingly, collapsing across cultures, 22 more disliked texture terms were mentioned than liked terms (Table S1)—a finding supporting the view that texture is often more salient when unpleasant (Szczesniak, [17]). Additionally, it has been stated that while flavor drives food liking, texture is often the food attribute responsible for food rejection (Cardello, [2]).

One cross‐cultural study, similar to ours, categorized each term by its main class of characteristics and showed that cultures similarly use more mechanical and geometrical terms (Nishinari et al., [11]). This notion may be true for our study as well; however, we asked a different question, "how does the number of main class of texture characteristics within a liked and disliked food combination culturally differ?" Here, we found differences across populations. The Asian (Singapore) population showed more main classes of characteristics within a liked than a disliked combination of textures. The opposite trend was found in the European (Polish) and North American populations. In this regard, foods with texture attributes representing distinct texture classes (i.e., mechanical, geometrical, other) tended to be aversive for the two Western cultures, but were reported as more palatable for the Asian population in our survey. This may reflect differences in the breadth of texture awareness across the three different cultures and future studies are needed to describe these differences in more detail and across a wider geographical population. It is important to note, however, that Singapore has a very diverse population in close proximity to distinct styles of cuisine across east Asia including influence from western cultures. Therefore, individuals in this environment are accustomed to many types of foods with varying texture.

Limitations

This study provides the consumer attitudes towards texture across three unique populations, however the authors made some methodological concessions to complete this study. The study was distributed online, meaning no actual food was consumed and the participants were not strictly controlled in the construction of their responses. Along these lines questions and survey instructions could have interpreted differently. Specifically, the respondents could have interpreted multiple textures differently. Additionally, the Singaporean population were university students. While the authors controlled for age in the statistical analyses, other sociocultural factors could be unique to the student population. Lastly, the Polish population was administered the survey in Polish, while the other populations completed the survey in English. There is a possibility that the available vocabulary for texture descriptors is different across the languages (i.e., Polish and English) used in this study.

CONCLUSION

This study highlights that texture combinations and contrasts are an important factor for texture acceptance across three cross‐cultural populations. The importance of texture combinations and contrasts was stable across populations, though textures with attributes from multiple main texture classes were associated with greater palatability in Asia and having multiple textures was found to be less important for food liking in the other two populations.

ACKNOWLEDGMENT

The authors would like to thank Li Ling Lee for assistance with survey preparation and data collection.

ETHICAL STATEMENTS

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Ethical Review

The study was conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the University of Tennessee IRB review for research involving human subjects (IRB # 17‐03973‐XP).

Informed Consent

All participants provided written informed consent and were compensated for their participation.

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS

Design (R.P & C.R.L.), acquisition of data (all authors), analysis (R.P. & C.R.L), interpretation (all authors), drafting/revising manuscript (all authors).

GRAPH: Supplementary Table S1 Supporting Information

Footnotes 1 This article was published on AA publication on: 06 November 2019 REFERENCES Blancher, G., Chollet, S., Kesteloot, R., Hoang, D. N., Cuvelier, G., & Sieffermann, J.‐M. (2007). French and Vietnamese: How do they describe texture characteristics of the same food? A case study with jellies. Food Quality and Preference, 18 (3), 560 – 575. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.FOODQUAL.2006.07.006 2 Cardello, A. V. (1996). The role of the human senses in food behavior. In Texture. Cereal foods world (USA) (p. II). Retrieved from http://agris.fao.org/agris-search/search.do?recordID=US9620352. 3 Cardello, A. V., Maller, O., Kapsalis, J. G., Segars, R. A., Sawyer, F. M., Murphy, C., & Moskowitz, H. R. (1982). Perception of texture by trained and consumer panelists. Journal of Food Science, 47 (4), 1186 – 1197. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2621.1982.tb07646.x 4 Drake, B. (1989). Sensory textural/rheological properties? A polyglot list. Journal of Texture Studies, 20 (1), 1 – 27. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-4603.1989.tb00417.x 5 Guinard, J. X., & Mazzucchelli, R. (1996). The sensory perception of texture and mouthfeel. Trends in Food Science and Technology, 7 (7), 213 – 219. https://doi.org/10.1016/0924-2244(96)10025-X 6 Hayakawa, F., Hatae, K., & Shimada, J. (2000). Characterization of onomatopoeia and mimetic languages of monthfeel. The Japanese Society for Food Science and Technology, 47 (3), 197 – 207. 7 Hayakawa, F., Ioku, K., Akuzawa, S., Saito, M., Nishinari, K., Yamano, Y., & Kohyama, K. (2005). Collection of Japanese texture terms (studies on Japanese texture terms part 1). The Japanese Society for Food Science and Technology, 52 (8), 337 – 346. 8 Hyde, R. J., & Witherly, S. A. (1993). Dynamic contrast: A sensory contribution to palatability. Appetite, 21, 1 – 16. https://doi.org/10.1006/appe.1993.1032 9 Lawless, H., Vanne, M., & Tuorila, H. (1997). Categorization of English and Finnish texture terms among consumers and food professionals. Journal of Texture Studies, 28 (6), 687 – 708. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-4603.1997.tb00147.x Luckett, C. R., & Seo, H.‐S. (2015). Consumer attitudes toward texture and other food attributes. Journal of Texture Studies, 46 (1), 46 – 57. https://doi.org/10.1111/jtxs.12110 Nishinari, K., Hayakawa, F., Xia, C., Huang, L., Meullenet, J., & Sieffermann, J. (2008). Comparative study of texture terms: English, French, Japanese and Chinese. Journal of Texture Studies, 39 (5), 530 – 568. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-4603.2008.00157.x Rohm, H. (1990). Consumer awareness of food texture in Austria. Journal of Texture Studies, 21 (3), 363 – 374. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-4603.1990.tb00485.x Szczesniak, A., & Kahn, E. L. (1984). Texture contrasts and combinations: A valued consumer attribute. Journal of Texture Studies, 15 (3), 285 – 301. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-4603.1984.tb00385.x Szczesniak, A. S. (1963). Classification of textural characteristics. Journal of Food Science, 28 (4), 385 – 389. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2621.1963.tb00215.x Szczesniak, A. S. (1971). Consumer awareness of texture and of other food attributes, II. Journal of Texture Studies, 2 (2), 196 – 206. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-4603.1971.tb00581.x Szczesniak, A. S. (1972). Consumer awareness of and attitudes to food texture ii. Children and teenagers. Journal of Texture Studies, 3 (2), 206 – 217. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-4603.1972.tb00624.x Szczesniak, A. S. (2002). Texture is a sensory property. Food Quality and Preference, 13, 215 – 225. Szczesniak, A. S., & Kahn, E. L. (1971). Consumer awareness of and attitudes to food texture. Journal of Texture Studies, 2 (3), 280 – 295. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-4603.1971.tb01005.x Szczesniak, A. S., & Kleyn, D. H. (1963). Consumer awareness of texture and of other food attributes. Food Technology, 17, 74 – 77. Yoshikawa, S., Nishimaru, S., Tashiro, T., & Yoshida, M. (1970). Collection and classification of words for description of food texture: Collection of words. Journal of Texture Studies, 1, 437 – 442.

By Robert Pellegrino; Bobby K. Cheon; Ciarán G. Forde; Anna Oleszkiewicz; Michal Pieniak and Curtis R. Luckett

Reported by Author; Author; Author; Author; Author; Author

Titel:
The contribution of texture contrasts and combinations to food acceptance across cultures.
Autor/in / Beteiligte Person: Pellegrino, R ; Cheon, BK ; Forde, CG ; Oleszkiewicz, A ; Pieniak, M ; Luckett, CR
Link:
Zeitschrift: Journal of texture studies, Jg. 51 (2020-04-01), Heft 2, S. 225-231
Veröffentlichung: 2005- : Oxford : Wiley ; <i>Original Publication</i>: Dordrecht, Reidel., 2020
Medientyp: academicJournal
ISSN: 1745-4603 (electronic)
DOI: 10.1111/jtxs.12494
Schlagwort:
  • Adolescent
  • Adult
  • Aged
  • Female
  • Humans
  • Male
  • Middle Aged
  • Poland
  • Singapore
  • Surveys and Questionnaires
  • United States
  • Young Adult
  • Cultural Characteristics
  • Feeding Behavior
  • Food Preferences
Sonstiges:
  • Nachgewiesen in: MEDLINE
  • Sprachen: English
  • Publication Type: Journal Article
  • Language: English
  • [J Texture Stud] 2020 Apr; Vol. 51 (2), pp. 225-231. <i>Date of Electronic Publication: </i>2019 Nov 25.
  • MeSH Terms: Cultural Characteristics* ; Feeding Behavior* ; Food Preferences* ; Adolescent ; Adult ; Aged ; Female ; Humans ; Male ; Middle Aged ; Poland ; Singapore ; Surveys and Questionnaires ; United States ; Young Adult
  • References: Blancher, G., Chollet, S., Kesteloot, R., Hoang, D. N., Cuvelier, G., & Sieffermann, J.-M. (2007). French and Vietnamese: How do they describe texture characteristics of the same food? A case study with jellies. Food Quality and Preference, 18(3), 560-575. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.FOODQUAL.2006.07.006. ; Cardello, A. V. (1996). The role of the human senses in food behavior. In Texture. Cereal foods world (USA) (p. II). Retrieved from http://agris.fao.org/agris-search/search.do?recordID=US9620352. ; Cardello, A. V., Maller, O., Kapsalis, J. G., Segars, R. A., Sawyer, F. M., Murphy, C., & Moskowitz, H. R. (1982). Perception of texture by trained and consumer panelists. Journal of Food Science, 47(4), 1186-1197. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2621.1982.tb07646.x. ; Drake, B. (1989). Sensory textural/rheological properties? A polyglot list. Journal of Texture Studies, 20(1), 1-27. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-4603.1989.tb00417.x. ; Guinard, J. X., & Mazzucchelli, R. (1996). The sensory perception of texture and mouthfeel. Trends in Food Science and Technology, 7(7), 213-219. https://doi.org/10.1016/0924-2244(96)10025-X. ; Hayakawa, F., Hatae, K., & Shimada, J. (2000). Characterization of onomatopoeia and mimetic languages of monthfeel. The Japanese Society for Food Science and Technology, 47(3), 197-207. ; Hayakawa, F., Ioku, K., Akuzawa, S., Saito, M., Nishinari, K., Yamano, Y., & Kohyama, K. (2005). Collection of Japanese texture terms (studies on Japanese texture terms part 1). The Japanese Society for Food Science and Technology, 52(8), 337-346. ; Hyde, R. J., & Witherly, S. A. (1993). Dynamic contrast: A sensory contribution to palatability. Appetite, 21, 1-16. https://doi.org/10.1006/appe.1993.1032. ; Lawless, H., Vanne, M., & Tuorila, H. (1997). Categorization of English and Finnish texture terms among consumers and food professionals. Journal of Texture Studies, 28(6), 687-708. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-4603.1997.tb00147.x. ; Luckett, C. R., & Seo, H.-S. (2015). Consumer attitudes toward texture and other food attributes. Journal of Texture Studies, 46(1), 46-57. https://doi.org/10.1111/jtxs.12110. ; Nishinari, K., Hayakawa, F., Xia, C., Huang, L., Meullenet, J., & Sieffermann, J. (2008). Comparative study of texture terms: English, French, Japanese and Chinese. Journal of Texture Studies, 39(5), 530-568. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-4603.2008.00157.x. ; Rohm, H. (1990). Consumer awareness of food texture in Austria. Journal of Texture Studies, 21(3), 363-374. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-4603.1990.tb00485.x. ; Szczesniak, A., & Kahn, E. L. (1984). Texture contrasts and combinations: A valued consumer attribute. Journal of Texture Studies, 15(3), 285-301. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-4603.1984.tb00385.x. ; Szczesniak, A. S. (1963). Classification of textural characteristics. Journal of Food Science, 28(4), 385-389. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2621.1963.tb00215.x. ; Szczesniak, A. S. (1971). Consumer awareness of texture and of other food attributes, II. Journal of Texture Studies, 2(2), 196-206. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-4603.1971.tb00581.x. ; Szczesniak, A. S. (1972). Consumer awareness of and attitudes to food texture ii. Children and teenagers. Journal of Texture Studies, 3(2), 206-217. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-4603.1972.tb00624.x. ; Szczesniak, A. S. (2002). Texture is a sensory property. Food Quality and Preference, 13, 215-225. ; Szczesniak, A. S., & Kahn, E. L. (1971). Consumer awareness of and attitudes to food texture. Journal of Texture Studies, 2(3), 280-295. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-4603.1971.tb01005.x. ; Szczesniak, A. S., & Kleyn, D. H. (1963). Consumer awareness of texture and of other food attributes. Food Technology, 17, 74-77. ; Yoshikawa, S., Nishimaru, S., Tashiro, T., & Yoshida, M. (1970). Collection and classification of words for description of food texture: Collection of words. Journal of Texture Studies, 1, 437-442.
  • Contributed Indexing: Keywords: combinations; contrasts; cross cultural; food preferences
  • Entry Date(s): Date Created: 20191108 Date Completed: 20210526 Latest Revision: 20210526
  • Update Code: 20240513

Klicken Sie ein Format an und speichern Sie dann die Daten oder geben Sie eine Empfänger-Adresse ein und lassen Sie sich per Email zusenden.

oder
oder

Wählen Sie das für Sie passende Zitationsformat und kopieren Sie es dann in die Zwischenablage, lassen es sich per Mail zusenden oder speichern es als PDF-Datei.

oder
oder

Bitte prüfen Sie, ob die Zitation formal korrekt ist, bevor Sie sie in einer Arbeit verwenden. Benutzen Sie gegebenenfalls den "Exportieren"-Dialog, wenn Sie ein Literaturverwaltungsprogramm verwenden und die Zitat-Angaben selbst formatieren wollen.

xs 0 - 576
sm 576 - 768
md 768 - 992
lg 992 - 1200
xl 1200 - 1366
xxl 1366 -