Zum Hauptinhalt springen

Concepts in Space: Enhancing Lexical Search With a Spatial Diversity Prime.

Malaie, S ; Karimi, H ; et al.
In: Cognitive science, Jg. 47 (2023-08-01), Heft 8, S. e13327
Online academicJournal

Titel:
Concepts in Space: Enhancing Lexical Search With a Spatial Diversity Prime.
Autor/in / Beteiligte Person: Malaie, S ; Karimi, H ; Jahanitabesh, A ; Bargh, JA ; Spivey, MJ
Link:
Zeitschrift: Cognitive science, Jg. 47 (2023-08-01), Heft 8, S. e13327
Veröffentlichung: 2009-: Hoboken, N.J. : Wiley-Blackwell ; <i>Original Publication</i>: Norwood, N. J., Ablex Pub. Corp., 2023
Medientyp: academicJournal
ISSN: 1551-6709 (electronic)
DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13327
Schlagwort:
  • Humans
  • Reaction Time
  • Language
  • Semantics
  • Cognition
  • Cues
Sonstiges:
  • Nachgewiesen in: MEDLINE
  • Sprachen: English
  • Publication Type: Journal Article
  • Language: English
  • [Cogn Sci] 2023 Aug; Vol. 47 (8), pp. e13327.
  • MeSH Terms: Cognition* ; Cues* ; Humans ; Reaction Time ; Language ; Semantics
  • References: Adelman, J. S., Brown, G. D., & Quesada, J. F. (2006). Contextual diversity, not word frequency, determines word-naming and lexical decision times. Psychological Science, 17(9), 814-823. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01787.x. ; Bargh, J.A. (2022). The cognitive unconscious in everyday life. In A. S. Reber & R. Allen (Eds.), The cognitive unconscious: The first half century (pp. 89-114). New York: Oxford University Press. ; Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(4), 577-660. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525×99002149. ; Barsalou, L. W. (2023). Implications of grounded cognition for conceptual processing across cultures. Topics in Cognitive Science. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12661. ; Beda, Z., & Smith, S. M. (2022). Unfixate your creative mind: Forgetting fixation and its applications. Translational Issues in Psychological Science, 8(1), 66. https://doi.org/10.1037/tps0000290. ; Brunyé, T. T., Gardony, A., Mahoney, C. R., & Taylor, H. A. (2012). Body-specific representations of spatial location. Cognition, 123(2), 229-239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.07.013. ; Casasanto, D., & Bottini, R. (2014). Spatial language and abstract concepts. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 5(2), 139-149. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1271. ; Chrysikou, E. G., Hamilton, R. H., Coslett, H. B., Datta, A., Bikson, M., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2013). Noninvasive transcranial direct current stimulation over the left prefrontal cortex facilitates cognitive flexibility in tool use. Cognitive Neuroscience, 4(2), 81-89. https://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2013.768221. ; Crawford, E. L., Margolies, S. M., Drake, J. T., & Murphy, M. E. (2006). Affect biases memory of location: Evidence for the spatial representation of affect. Cognition & Emotion, 20(8), 1153-1169. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930500347794. ; Dehkhoda, A. (1998). Dehkhoda dictionary. Tehran, Iran: University of Tehran, Dehkhoda Dictionary Institute. ; Domahs, F., Moeller, K., Huber, S., Willmes, K., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2010). Embodied numerosity: Implicit hand-based representations influence symbolic number processing across cultures. Cognition, 116(2), 251-266. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.05.007. ; Ehrlichman, H., & Micic, D. (2012). Why do people move their eyes when they think? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(2), 96-100. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721412436810. ; Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/4737.001.0001. ; Gick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1983). Schema induction and analogical reasoning. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 1-38. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(83)90002-6. ; Gibbs, R. W. Jr., & Matlock, T. (2008). Metaphor, imagination, and simulation: Psycholinguistic evidence. In R. W. Gibbs Jr. (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of metaphor and thought (pp. 161-176). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511816802.011. ; Grant, E. R., & Spivey, M. J. (2003). Eye movements and problem solving: Guiding attention guides thought. Psychological Science, 14(5), 462-466. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.02454. ; Guerra, E., & Knoeferle, P. (2014). Spatial distance effects on incremental semantic interpretation of abstract sentences: Evidence from eye tracking. Cognition, 133(3), 535-552. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.07.007. ; Guerra, E., & Knoeferle, P. (2017). Visually perceived spatial distance affects the interpretation of linguistically mediated social meaning during online language comprehension: An eye tracking reading study. Journal of Memory and Language, 92, 43-56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2016.05.004. ; Hills, T. (2006). Animal foraging and the evolution of goal-directed cognition. Cognitive Science, 30, 3-41. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0000_50. ; Hills, T. T., Todd, P. M., & Goldstone, R. L. (2008). Search in external and internal spaces: Evidence for generalized cognitive search processes. Psychological Science, 19(8), 802-808. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02160.x. ; Hills, T. T., Todd, P. M., & Goldstone, R. L. (2010). The central executive as a search process: Priming exploration and exploitation across domains. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(4), 590. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020666. ; James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Holt. ; JASP Team (2019). JASP (Version 0.9.2)[Computer software]. Accessed April 8, 2023 at: https://jasp-stats.org/. ; Kello, C. T., Brown, G. D.A., Ferrer-i-Cancho, R., Holden, J. G., Linkenkaer-Hansen, K., Rhodes, T., & Van Orden, G. C. (2010). Scaling laws in cognitive sciences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(5), 223-232. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.02.005. ; Knops, A., Thirion, B., Hubbard, E. M., Michel, V., & Dehaene, S. (2009). Recruitment of an area involved in eye movements during mental arithmetic. Science, 324(5934), 1583-1585. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1171599. ; Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. https://doi.org/10.7208/chicago/9780226470993.001.0001. ; Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). A solution to Plato's problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104(2), 211-240. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.104.2.211. ; Locke, E. A. (2015). Theory building, replication, and behavioral priming: Where do we need to go from here? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(3), 408-414. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691614567231. ; Louwerse, M. M., & Zwaan, R. A. (2009). Language encodes geographical information. Cognitive Science, 33(1), 51-73. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2008.01003.x. ; Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A. (2008). A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content. Journal of Physiology-Paris, 102(1-3), 59-70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.03.004. ; Meier, B. P., & Robinson, M. D. (2004). Why the sunny side is up: Associations between affect and vertical position. Psychological Science, 15(4), 243-247. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000062. ; Mirman, D., & Magnuson, J. S. (2008). Attractor dynamics and semantic neighborhood density: Processing is slowed by near neighbors and speeded by distant neighbors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(1), 65-79. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.34.1.65. ; Montez, P., Thompson, G., & Kello, C. T. (2015). The role of semantic clustering in optimal memory foraging. Cognitive Science, 39(8), 1925-1939. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12249. ; Pedone, R., Hummel, J. E., & Holyoak, K. J. (2001). The use of diagrams in analogical problem solving. Memory & Cognition, 29(2), 214-221. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194915. ; Pinhas, M., Shaki, S., & Fischer, M. H. (2014). Heed the signs: Operation signs have spatial associations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1527-1540. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2014.892516. ; Rhodes, T., & Turvey, M. T. (2007). Human memory retrieval as Lévy foraging. Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications, 385(1), 255-260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physa.2007.07.001. ; Roseman, L., Leech, R., Feilding, A., Nutt, D. J., & Carhart-Harris, R. L. (2014). The effects of psilocybin and MDMA on between-network resting state functional connectivity in healthy volunteers. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 204. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00204. ; Sherman, J. W., & Rivers, A. M. (2021a). There's nothing social about social priming: Derailing the “train wreck.” Psychological Inquiry, 32(1), 1-11. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2021.1889312. ; Sherman, J. W., & Rivers, A. M. (2021b). A final word on train wrecks. Psychological Inquiry, 32(1), 49-52. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2021.1889845. ; Simpson, J. A., & Weiner, E. S. C. (1989). The Oxford English dictionary. Oxford, England: Clarendon Press. ; Spivey, M. J. (2007). The continuity of mind. New York: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195170788.001.0001. ; Spivey, M. J., & Geng, J. J. (2001). Oculomotor mechanisms activated by imagery and memory: Eye movements to absent objects. Psychological Research, 65(4), 235-241. https://doi.org/10.1007/s004260100059. ; Spivey, M. J., Tyler, M. J., Richardson, D. C., & Young, E. E. (2000). Eye movements during comprehension of spoken descriptions. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Mahwah, NJ (pp. 487-492). ; Taylor, N. L., D'Souza, A., Munn, B. R., Lv, J., Zaborszky, L., Müller, E. J., Wainstein, G., Calamante, F., & Shine, J. M. (2022). Structural connections between the noradrenergic and cholinergic system shape the dynamics of functional brain networks. NeuroImage, 260, 119455. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119455. ; Thomas, L. E., & Lleras, A. (2007). Moving eyes and moving thought: On the spatial compatibility between eye movements and cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(4), 663-668. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196818. ; Thomas, L. E., & Lleras, A. (2009). Covert shifts of attention function as an implicit aid to insight. Cognition, 111(2), 168-174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.01.005. ; Todd, P. M., & Hills, T. T. (2020). Foraging in mind. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(3), 309-315. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721420915861. ; van Doorn, J., van den Bergh, D., Böhm, U., Dablander, F., Derks, K., Draws, T., Etz, A., Evans, N., Gronau, Q., Haaf, J., Hinne, M., Kucharsky, S., Ly, A., Marsman, M., Matzke, D., Gupta, A., Sarafoglou, A., Stefan, A., Voelkel, J., & Wagenmakers, E. J. (2021). The JASP guidelines for conducting and reporting a Bayesian analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 28, 813-826. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-020-01798-5. ; Weingarten, E., Chen, Q., McAdams, M., Yi, J., Hepler, J., & Albarracín, D. (2016). From primed concepts to action: A meta-analysis of the behavioral effects of incidentally presented words. Psychological Bulletin, 142(5), 472-497. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000030. ; Werner, K., Raab, M., & Fischer, M. H. (2019). Moving arms: The effects of sensorimotor information on the problem-solving process. Thinking & Reasoning, 25(2), 171-191. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2018.1494630. ; Williams, L. E., Huang, J. Y., & Bargh, J. A. (2009). The scaffolded mind: Higher mental processes are grounded in early experience of the physical world. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39(7), 1257-1267. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.665.
  • Contributed Indexing: Keywords: Conceptual system; Embodied cognition; Physical and mental search processes; Spatial relations
  • Entry Date(s): Date Created: 20230803 Date Completed: 20230804 Latest Revision: 20230806
  • Update Code: 20240513

Klicken Sie ein Format an und speichern Sie dann die Daten oder geben Sie eine Empfänger-Adresse ein und lassen Sie sich per Email zusenden.

oder
oder

Wählen Sie das für Sie passende Zitationsformat und kopieren Sie es dann in die Zwischenablage, lassen es sich per Mail zusenden oder speichern es als PDF-Datei.

oder
oder

Bitte prüfen Sie, ob die Zitation formal korrekt ist, bevor Sie sie in einer Arbeit verwenden. Benutzen Sie gegebenenfalls den "Exportieren"-Dialog, wenn Sie ein Literaturverwaltungsprogramm verwenden und die Zitat-Angaben selbst formatieren wollen.

xs 0 - 576
sm 576 - 768
md 768 - 992
lg 992 - 1200
xl 1200 - 1366
xxl 1366 -