Zum Hauptinhalt springen

Cognitive processes during deception about attitudes revisited: a replication study

Scheuble, Vera ; Beauducel, André
In: Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, Jg. 15 (2020-01-24), Heft 8
Online unknown

Cognitive processes during deception about attitudes revisited: a replication study 

Event-related potential (ERP) studies about deception often apply recognition tasks. It remains questionable whether reported ERP patterns and cognitive processes can be generalized to other contexts. As the study by Johnson et al. (2008) fills this gap by investigating deception regarding attitudes, we tried to replicate it. Participants (N  = 99) were instructed to lie or tell the truth about their attitudes. We obtained the same results as Johnson et al. (2008): lies were accompanied by enhanced medial frontal negativities (MFN) and suppressed late positive components (LPCs) indicating that lying relied on stronger cognitive control processes and response conflicts than being honest. The amplitudes of pre-response positivities (PRP) were reduced for lies implying that lies about attitudes were accompanied by strategic monitoring. MFN amplitudes increased and LPC amplitudes decreased for lies about positively valued items revealing that lying about positively valued items is cognitively more challenging than lying about negatively valued items. As a new finding, MFN, LPC and PRP components were neither moderated by Machiavellianism nor by changes in the attitude ratings. The results indicate that LPC, MFN and PRP components are reliable indicators of the cognitive processes used during deception and that it is worthwhile to investigate them in further deception contexts.

Keywords: deception; MFN; LPC; attitudes; Machiavellianism

Introduction

ERP components in deception studies

Recently, many studies have been focused on the potential of ERP components to detect deception. Most of them apply recognition tasks in a forensic context. Studies investigating deception in other contexts are scarce (Leue and Beauducel, 2019). Yet, to get to know cognitive processes underlying deception in general, it is mandatory to study deception in different contexts. One promising study investigating deception about attitudes is by Johnson et al. (2008). They found large effects of at least ηp2 = 0.63 analyzing the difference between deception and honest responses for late positive components (LPC), medial frontal negativities (MFN) and a component they named pre-response positivity (PRP). The importance of reproducible psychological studies has been emphasized (Yong, 2012; Open Science Collaboration, 2015). We aimed to replicate the research findings of Johnson et al. (2008) to help set a solid basis for future ERP studies about lies beyond the recognition and forensic contexts. The study was pre-registered before data collection on the Open Science Framework (link to pre-registration: https://osf.io/f6w97).

The MFN is a negative deflection occurring up to 100 ms after a response at fronto-central electrodes (Johnson et al. 2008). Its neural source is supposed to be the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) or areas nearby (Gehring and Willoughby, 2004; Nieuwenhuis et al. 2004). Activity of the ACC has been associated with response inhibition and monitoring of conflicting response tendencies (Aron et al. 2004; Botvinick, 2007). Likewise, telling a lie relies on inhibiting the truth and formulating another answer; conflicts have to be monitored and solved (Gombos, 2006; Sip et al. 2008). In ERP studies, larger MFN amplitudes occurred for deceptive compared to truthful responses (Johnson et al. 2004; Kireev et al. 2008; Leue et al. 2012; Gibbons et al. 2018; Scheuble and Beauducel, 2020). Johnson et al. (2004, 2008) reported increased MFN amplitudes for lies about attitudes and regarding the familiarity of items. Likewise, Kireev et al. (2008) found a more negative amplitude of an ERP component similar to the MFN, for deception in a card game. Conversely, Suchotzki et al. (2015) found larger MFNs for truthful than for deceptive responses applying the Sheffield lie test. In summary, the evidence for larger MFNs for deceptive responses in other than recognition contexts relies heavily on the study by Johnson et al. (2008).

Another ERP component closely related to deception is the LPC, sometimes also named P300 (Johnson et al. 2003, 2004, 2005, 2008; Meijer et al. 2007; Polich, 2007). Since the LPC and P300 have not been associated with invariant features and these terms have been used interchangeably in the deception literature, we apply the term LPC in the following (Polich, 2007, p. 2128; Leue and Beauducel, 2019). According to Johnson's triarchic model (1986, 1988), LPC amplitudes are affected by stimulus probability, stimulus meaning, and how much of the transmitted stimulus information is received. The majority of deception studies rely on its feature to indicate stimulus probability. When a person recognizes an item in a series of other unknown items, it is perceived as more infrequent than the unknown items and therefore accompanied by a more positive LPC amplitude, even when the recognition of the stimulus is denied (Rosenfeld et al. 2013; Leue and Beauducel, 2019). Some studies, however, studied deception that did not rely on the recognition of an item (Leue and Beauducel, 2019). When attention is drawn away from a stimulus by another task, decreased LPCs can be found, since transmission of the stimulus information is hampered (Johnson, 1986, 1988; Palmer et al. 1994; Beauducel et al. 2006). Lying could be considered as such a dual-task (Johnson et al. 2003, 2004, 2005, 2008). Correspondingly, in these studies suppressed LPC amplitudes occurred for deceptive compared to honest responses (Vendemia and Buzan, 2005; Dong et al. 2010; Meek et al. 2013; Pfister et al. 2014). Johnson et al. (2008) observed this pattern of LPCs for lies about attitudes. Furthermore, it has been observed for lies about the evaluation of attractiveness, lies about known facts, and knowledge received during an examination session (Vendemia and Buzan, 2005; Dong et al. 2010; Meek et al. 2013; Pfister et al. 2014). However, to improve our knowledge on cognitive processes during lies, further studies applying deception tasks in multiple contexts have to be conducted and reliable results have to be found. Replicating the results of prior studies, like those of Johnson et al. (2008), is a step towards this goal.

Johnson et al. (2004, 2005, 2008) also analyzed a response-synchronized ERP component preceding a response, which they named PRP. Decreased PRP amplitudes have been related by Johnson et al. (2004, 2005, 2008) to strategic monitoring and 'processing required to make intention-based responses' (Johnson , 2014, p. 254). Strategic, higher-order monitoring processes ensure that a long-term goal is kept in mind and responses align with this goal. Conversely, tactical monitoring defines cognitive control processes that continue over a short period of time, like the time between a stimulus presentation and a response (Johnson et al. 2004, 2005, 2008). In a previous study by Johnson et al. (2004, 2005), participants completed a recognition task. Tactical monitoring was required when they were instructed to lie on every trial (directed lies). In a different task, strategic monitoring was needed when participants chose on their own to lie or not, but were instructed to give in total an equal amount of deceptive and honest responses (self-generated lies). PRP amplitudes were slightly attenuated for directed lies, but highly attenuated for self-generated lies requiring strategic monitoring (Johnson et al. 2004, 2005). Conversely, for directed lies about attitudes Johnson et al. (2008) found a decrease of PRP amplitudes similar to the decrease for self-generated lies. They concluded that lies about attitudes are accompanied by strategic monitoring. Due to the fact that the PRP component occurs before a response, it is possible that it temporally overlaps with a contingent negative variation (slow negative potential between a warning stimuli and a response signal) or the Bereitschaftspotential (slow negative shift preceding a response), if either of them is present. There are, to our knowledge, no deception studies from other researchers analyzing the PRP component. Overall, this motivates the replication of this relatively new and promising ERP component of deception studies. Although Tu et al. (2009) also analyzed lies about attitudes, it is hard to draw a conclusion about the reproducibility of the findings by Johnson et al. (2008) from their study because they applied a different task and analyzed stimulus locked ERP components (N400–700 and P1000–2000).

Johnson et al. (2008) found that lying about positively valued (agree) items was more conflicting and required more cognitive resources than lying about negatively valued (disagree) items: MFN amplitudes were enhanced and LPC amplitudes were attenuated for deceptive responses of agree items. Furthermore, PRPs were smaller for agree than for disagree items. They reasoned that lying about positively valued topics could be perceived as a denial of the self and is accordingly accompanied by more conflict. Moreover, they explained that lying about negatively rated items could be seen as a form of compliance. In everyday life, overrating the positive aspects of a negatively valued topic is probably often perceived as socially more acceptable than saying the truth. In a study by Gershoff et al. (2008), people overestimated the population consensus to a greater extent for items they liked than for items they disliked. Furthermore, they reported that it is easier for people to recall positive features of negatively rated items than vice versa. The importance of positive–negative asymmetries has been underlined in different domains and is predictive for judgments and related behavior (Fazio et al. 2015). Since the recognition of negative aspects from positively valued topics seems to be more challenging and people rather overestimate the population consensus for themes they like, lying about positively valued items should require more cognitive control processes resulting in larger effects on ERP components for positively than negatively valued items. Even though this effect was already shown in Johnson et al. (2008), they did not expect effects of the valence of the items and a replication of it was especially needed.

Deception and Machiavellianism

Results of different studies, and findings from deception studies in particular, imply that MFNs and LPCs can be modulated by personality traits. Individuals scoring higher in justice sensitivity showed larger differences for early LPC as well as MFN amplitudes between deceptive and honest responses in a study by Leue et al. (2012). Machiavellianism is characterized by a manipulative and strategic behavior, a cynical worldview, callous affect and a lack of morality (Jones and Paulhus, 2014). Individuals scoring higher on Machiavellianism deceive more frequently, deceiving seems to be cognitively less strenuous for them, and they rate their abilities in deceiving higher (Kashy and DePaulo, 1996; Gozna et al. 2001; Azizli et al. 2016; Wissing and Reinhard, 2019). Scheuble and Beauducel (2020) found a moderating effect of Machiavellianism on MFNs for female witnesses: The difference between MFN amplitudes of deceptive compared to honest responses was smaller for women scoring higher on Machiavellianism. Likewise, Panasiti et al. (2014) reported a lower BP before lying for participants higher in Machiavellianism. To sum up people high in Machiavellianism seem to have less scruple to lie, which could go along with a different cognitive processing of lying. Hence, additionally to replicating Johnson et al. (2008), we elucidated moderating effects of Machiavellianism.

Aims and hypotheses

Lying should be accompanied by more response conflicts and require more cognitive control processes, resulting in (a) larger MFN and (b) attenuated LPC amplitudes for lies compared to honest responses. Attitudes shape many behaviors and strategic monitoring can help to act in line with one's lies. Accordingly, we expected (c) attenuated PRP amplitudes for lies compared to honest responses. Lying about negatively valued items should be cognitively less challenging, since it can be perceived as a form of compliance and it is easier for people to recognize positive features for negatively valued themes. Therefore, we expected (d) larger MFN and (e) suppressed LPC amplitudes for deception of agree compared to deception of disagree items. Furthermore, we expected PRPs to be (f) attenuated for agree compared to disagree items, since people tend to overestimate the population consensus for positively valued themes resulting in greater strategic monitoring in order to behave compliant to them. Additional to these effects, which were found by Johnson et al. (2008), we investigated moderating effects of Machiavellianism. Since people higher in Machiavellianism tend to lie more frequently, we expected that lying would be cognitively less strenuous for them resulting in (g) smaller differences of MFN as well as (h) LPC amplitudes between deceptive and honest responses. The associations of Machiavellianism with PRP amplitudes were explored.

Method

Participants

We collected data from 120 participants (see Supplement S2 for a description of the determination of the sample size). As in the study by Johnson et al. (2008), participants with less than 16 artifact free and correct trials in one of the analyzed categories (agree lie, agree honest, disagree lie, disagree honest) were excluded from analysis (n = 20). An additional participant had to be excluded because her response times of catch trials indicated that she likely reversed button assignments during the deception task1 (cf. Johnson et al. 2008). A final sample of 99 participants (50 male; age: M = 21.28, SD = 2.80, range: 18–32 years) was available for analysis. Participants were right-handed, native German speakers, had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, did not take drugs or psychoactive medication and none of them had neurological, psychological or emotional disorders (cf. Johnson et al. 2008). All participants signed written informed consent before the examination and participated voluntarily. Psychology students (40%) got course credit for their participation. The study was performed in accordance with the revised Helsinki declaration (2013). The local ethic board of the Institute of Psychology from the University of Bonn approved the study.

Measures

The effects of different conceptions of Machiavellianism were controlled for by applying two different scales: an older German scale based on the questionnaire of Christie and Geis (1970) and a more recent scale (Henning and Six, 1977; Jones and Paulhus, 2014). Moreover, a total scale was formed as an equally weighted aggregate of the two scales. The internal consistency of the scale by Henning and Six (1977) was acceptable (α = 0.77), just like the internal consistency of the scale by Jones and Paulhus (α = 0.75). The internal consistency of the aggregate of both scales was moderate (α = 0.84).

The attitude questionnaire by Johnson et al. (2008) consists of items referring to religious, political and moral themes, well known people, and preferences. Some items are difficult to understand for German participants because they are based on content that is scarcely known in the German culture. Therefore, we excluded 12 (e.g. LASAR requirements, MEDICARE, workfare) of the 118 items of Johnson et al. (2008). Furthermore, we added items that are relevant for Germans nowadays (e.g. AfD, Angela Merkel, veganism; see Supplement S1). The final questionnaire consisted of 183 items. Participants were instructed to indicate how much they agree with the items, how strongly their attitudes are held, and how important their attitudes are. Responses were given on scales from one (extremely unfavorable/disagree; not at all strongly; not at all important) to seven (extremely favorable/agree; extremely strongly; extremely important). For the deception task, we chose for each participant 26–30 items with the highest agreement (rated as 7, 6) as well as disagreement ratings (rated as 1, 2), selecting at first items that were also highly rated on the strength scale (Johnson et al. 2008). Johnson et al. (2008) also included 26–30 neutral items for each participant (rated as 4, 3 or 5 in the agreeableness scale). However, since people could have either ambivalent or no feelings towards them, they were not analyzed (Johnson et al. 2008). As the exclusion of these items might alter the cognitive processes during the task, we also included them into the deception task. Participants completed the attitude questionnaire once again after the deception task. This gave us the opportunity to control for changes in the attitudes.

Deception task

We contacted Johnson, who kindly gave us additional information about the study (e.g. instructions of the deception task, items of the attitude questionnaire). All instructions and procedures were kept as close as possible to the original study. Participants saw stimuli on a computer screen and reacted to them by pressing either the left or the right of two buttons. During the deception task, participants were instructed to indicate whether they agree or disagree with the items selected from the attitude questionnaire (Johnson et al. 2008). For neutral items, participants were instructed to indicate their tendency. During one block, participants were instructed to respond honestly and in another block they were instructed to lie about their attitudes (Johnson et al. 2008). Each block consisted of 108 trials split into 18 catch trails and 90 trials with attitude items (Johnson et al. 2008). For catch trials, the words of the button assignments ('Agree' and 'Disagree') were presented on the screen and participants should respond to them with the corresponding button press, meaning, for example, pressing the button that stood for 'I agree with the item' when the word 'Agree' appeared (Johnson et al. 2008). Catch trials were included to ensure that participants did not simplify the task by swapping the meaning of the response buttons when they had to lie. The order of the two parts and the assignment of response buttons were counterbalanced across participants (Johnson et al. 2008). As in Johnson et al. (2008), the stimulus word was shown for 400 ms followed by an inter-stimulus interval with a randomized duration of 2450–2850 ms (Figure 1). Participants could react to the stimulus word immediately or during the subsequent inter stimulus interval.

Graph: Fig. 1 class="chapter-para">Sequence of two trials of the deception task.

Procedure

Participants completed the attitude questionnaire around 1 week (M = 6.87, SD = 1.07) before the EEG examination (cf. Johnson et al. 2008). The EEG examination took part in a sound attenuated, electrically shielded and well-lit room. Participants sat about 95 cm away from a 19″ flat screen. The deception task was presented through Presentation V20.1 (Neurobehavioral Systems). It began with a simple orthographic task, so that participants could get to know the trial sequence (Johnson et al. 2008). As in Johnson's study, participants should discriminate two phonologically similar words: Hafen (haven) and Hafer (oat). Subsequently, participants completed the deception task.

EEG recording and quantification

The EEG was recorded by an ActiveTwo BioSemi system (BioSemi, Amsterdam, Netherlands). Sixty-four active Ag/AgCl scalp electrodes were placed according to the extended 10/20 system (Jasper, 1958). Two additional electrodes were located at the pre-auricular sides. An electrooculogram (EOG) was formed by one electrode located at the middle of the forehead (FP1) and one electrode placed 2 cm below the outer canthus of the left eye (Johnson et al. 2008). As per Biosemi design, the Common Mode Sense active electrode and the Driven Right Leg passive electrode served as ground electrodes. Signals were digitized using ActiView software (BioSemi). The EEG was recorded with a sampling rate of 128 Hz, which is close to the sampling rate of 100 Hz used in Johnson et al. (2008). Offline analyses were performed with EEGLab (version 12.0.2.6b; Delorme and Makeig, 2004), based on MATLAB 7.14.739 (The MathWorks, Natick, MA). Data were re-referenced to averaged pre-auricular sides, filtered applying a 0.01–35 Hz band-pass filter, and segmented into epochs ranging from 1150 ms before until 300 ms after the response (baseline: −1150 to −1000 ms; Johnson et al. 2008). Eye movements were removed by excluding epochs in which the signal of the EOG during any eight consecutive sampling points of the epoch exceeded 50 μV (cf. Johnson et al. 2008). ERP components were quantified as mean amplitudes. MFNs were measured between 10 and 80 ms and PRPs between −100 and 0 ms at Fz, FC1, FC2 and Cz (Johnson et al. 2008). LPCs were measured between −100 and +100 ms at P3, Pz, P4, CP1 and CP2 (Johnson et al. 2008). Grand averages of fronto-central and parietal-central electrodes are depicted in Figures 2 and 3.

Graph: Fig. 2 class="chapter-para">Response-locked grand averages of fronto-central electrodes for lies as well as truthful responses for items participants agreed and disagreed to. Epochs spanned from 1150 ms before until 300 ms after the response. One tick at the x-axis stands for 100 ms and one tick at the y-axis for 1 μV.

Statistical analysis

Conventional statistical analyses were conducted with IBM SPSS (Version 24). Percentage of correct responses and reaction times served as behavioral data. For the percentage of correct responses, responses to attitude items that corresponded to the instructions (pressing the agree/disagree button for items one agreed with in the honest/deceptive block) were compared to those that did not correspond to the instructions (pressing the disagree/agree button for items one agreed with in the honest/deceptive block and missing responses). Repeated measures ANOVAs were computed for behavioral data and amplitude data for the LPC, MFN and PRP ERP components with Response (honest vs deception) and Valence (agree vs disagree) as within subject factors (Johnson et al. 2008). The ANOVAs of ERP data also included Electrode Position as a within subject factor (Johnson et al. 2008). To test whether LPC and MFN amplitudes differed among deceptive responses for agree and disagree items (hypothesis d and e), repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted including only deceptive responses (Johnson et al. 2008). Separate repeated measures ANCOVAs were computed that included additionally the mean centered Machiavellianism scores as a covariate and Response × Machiavellianism as an interaction term. A description of the calculation of change scores for the attitude ratings are given in Supplement S3. Effects of violations of the sphericity assumption were corrected by means of Greenhouse–Geisser epsilon for the degrees of freedom and partial eta2 is reported as an effect size (Johnson et al. 2008). Only two-tailed levels of statistical significance are reported. Furthermore, we calculated Bayes factors using JASP (Version 0.10.0.0; JASP Team; 2018) because—in contrast to frequentist statistics—Bayes factors also allow for an interpretation of null results. We report BF10, which represents a ratio of the likelihood of our data under assumption of the alternative hypothesis and the likelihood of our data under assumption of the null hypothesis. For instance, a Bayes factor of 10.00 indicates that the data are 10 times more likely to occur under the alternative than under the null hypothesis. According to Jeffreys (1961) and Lee and Wagenmakers (2013), a Bayes factor of at least 3 indicates evidence for the alternative hypothesis and a Bayes factor of at least 100 indicates decisive evidence for the alternative hypothesis. A Bayes factor smaller than 1/3 indicates evidence for the null hypothesis. JASP does not calculate Bayes factors of single interaction terms. Therefore, Bayes factors of interaction terms were obtained by computing the differences between the ERP components for truthful and deceptive responses and using them as the dependent variable in the repeated measures Bayesian ANOVAs. The calculated Bayes factors of the main effects of these ANOVAs represent the interaction terms with the Response variable (truthful vs deceptive responses).

We additionally conducted repeated measures ANOVAs of ERP components considering the data of all participants (without exclusion criteria). Categorization in significant results was the same as for the following reported analyses (see Supplement S4). Furthermore, additional bootstrap analyses served to calculate the accuracy of detecting deception through patterns of LPC, MFN and PRP amplitudes (cf. Rosenfeld et al. 1991; Olson et al. 2018). Methods and results of them can be found in Supplements S5 and S6.

Results

Ratings for agree and disagree items

The descriptive statistics for agree, disagree and neutral items of the deception task from the present study and the study by Johnson et al. (2008) are summarized in Table 1. The evaluation, strength and importance ratings are comparable to those of Johnson et al. (2008). Agree items were evaluated significantly more positive than disagree items, t(98) = 128.08, P < 0.001. Agreement and disagreement in topics differed in strength and importance with higher ratings for disagree items, tstrength(98) = −4.11, Pstrength < 0.001; timportance(98) = −3.62, Pimportance < 0.001. The differences in the corresponding means were similar, although a bit smaller, for the present study compared to the study by Johnson et al. (2008).

Table 1 Ratings of the agree, disagree and neutral items of the deception task

. Present study. Johnson et al. (2008).
Evaluation. Strength. Importance. Evaluation. Strength. Importance.
Agree 6.69 (0.29) 6.21 (0.63) 5.47 (0.85) 6.50 (0.35) 5.63 (1.06) 5.37 (1.09)
Disagree 1.12 (0.19) 6.42 (0.64) 5.73 (0.93) 1.23 (0.24) 6.03 (0.68) 5.68 (0.90)
Neutral 4.00 (0.03) 3.37 (1.17) 2.60 (0.93) 4.18 (0.60) 2.82 (0.95) 2.59 (0.94)

Notes: Means (SD) for the evaluation, strength and importance ratings of the present study and the study by Johnson et al. (2008). Ratings were given on a scale from 1 (=disagree, not at all important/strongly) to 7 (=agree, extremely important/strongly).

Behavioral data

The percentage of correct responses was higher for honest (M = 95.83%, SE = 0.37) compared to deceptive responses (M = 90.49%, SE = 0.59), F(1, 98) = 103.19, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.51, BF10 = 1.06e+20. Neither the main effect of Valence nor the interaction of Response × Valence were significant for the percentage of correct responses (all Ps > 0.14, BF10, Valence = 0.14, BF10, Response × Valence = 0.43).

The repeated measures ANOVAs of response times revealed that responses were slower for deceptive than for honest responses (Table 2), F(1, 98) = 241.46, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.71, BF10 = 3.43e+65. Furthermore, responses were slower for disagree than for agree items (Table 2), F(1, 98) = 18.79, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.16. Yet, the corresponding Bayes factor suggested no evidence for the alternative hypothesis, BF10 = 0.38. The Response × Valence interaction was significant, F(1, 98) = 15.77, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.14, BF10 = 143.81. The main effect of Valence was significant for truthful but not for deceptive responses, Ftruthful(1, 98) = 41.39, Ptruthful < 0.001, ηp2truthful = 0.30, BF10, truthful = 1.86e+6; Fdeception(1, 98) = 0.66, Pdeception = 0.42, ηp2deception = 0.01, BF10, deception = 0.20.

Table 2 Mean (SD) reaction times in ms for truthful responses and lies of agree and disagree items

. Truthful. Lie.
Agree 881.46 (181.21) 1220.97 (275.93)
Disagree 943.68 (194.92) 1230.26 (283.40)

Graph: Fig. 3 class="chapter-para">Response-locked grand averages of parietal-central electrodes for lies as well as truthful responses for items participants agreed and disagreed to. Epochs spanned from 1150 ms before until 300 ms after the response. One tick at the x-axis stands for 100 ms and one tick at the y-axis for 1 μV.

Repeated measures ANCOVAs that additionally considered Machiavellianism scores yielded no significant interactions between Response (honest vs deceptive response) and Machiavellianism neither for response times nor for percentage of correct responses (all Ps > 0.68, BF10 between 0.21 and 0.38).

MFN amplitudes

Repeated measures ANOVAs yielded a significant effect of Electrode position, F(1.71, 167.07) = 30.08, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.24, BF10 = 1.09e+6. Simple contrasts revealed that MFN amplitudes were larger at Fz (M = −0.98 μV, SE = 0.45), than at FC1 (M = −0.51 μV, SE = 0.46; F(1, 98) = 5.02, P < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.05), FC2 (M = −0.03 μV, SE = 0.47; F(1, 98) = 20.57, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.17), and Cz (M = 1.49 μV, SE = 0.50; F(1, 98) = 39.09, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.29). In accordance with hypothesis a, MFN amplitudes were larger for deceptive than for honest responses (Figure 4A), F(1, 98) = 28.10, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.22. The corresponding Bayes factor suggested decisive evidence for hypothesis a, BF10 = 8.12e+18. Moreover, a significant main effect of Valence revealed that larger MFN amplitudes occurred for agree than for disagree items (Figure 4B), F(1, 98) = 6.98, P < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.07, BF10 = 3139.63. The Response × Valence interaction was not significant, F(1, 98) = 2.74, P = 0.10, ηp2 = 0.03, although the Bayes factor provided evidence against the null hypothesis, BF10 = 95.17. Corresponding with hypothesis d, the Valence effect was significant for lies (Figure 4C), F(1, 98) = 6.70, P < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.06, BF10 = 305984.01, but not for honest responses, F(1, 98) = 0.52, P = 0.47, ηp2 = 0.01, BF10 = 0.22.

Graph: Fig. 4 class="chapter-para">Means and standard errors of MFN amplitudes representing (A) the Response main effect (lies vs truthful responses), (B) the Valence effect (agree vs disagree items) and (C) the Valence effect for lies (lies for agree vs lies for disagree items).

PRP amplitudes

A significant main effect of Electrode position occurred for PRP amplitudes, F(1.76, 172.41) = 22.74, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.19, BF10 = 2898.53. PRP amplitudes were smaller at Fz (M = 0.98 μV, SE = 0.39) compared to FC2 (M = 1.56 μV, SE = 0.42; F(1, 98) = 8.54, P < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.08) and Cz (M = 2.89 μV, SE = 0.47; F(1, 98) = 28.74, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.23). As expected in hypothesis c, PRP amplitudes were suppressed for deceptive compared to honest responses (Figure 5A), F(1, 98) = 49.12, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.33, which was strongly supported by the Bayes factor, BF10 = 1.85e+33. Furthermore, the main effect of Valence was significant (Figure 5B), F(1, 98) = 12.02, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.11, BF10 = 1.57e+6. Corresponding with hypothesis f, PRP amplitudes were smaller for agree than for disagree items. The interaction of Response × Valence was not significant, F(1, 98) = 1.51, P = 0.22, ηp2 = 0.02, whereas the corresponding Bayes factor suggested some evidence for the alternative hypothesis, BF10 = 3.60.

Graph: Fig. 5 class="chapter-para">Means and standard errors of PRP amplitudes representing (A) the Response main effect (lies vs truthful responses) and (B) the Valence effect (agree vs disagree items).

LPC amplitudes

LPC amplitudes differed significantly depending on Electrode position, F(2.64, 258.28) = 14.69, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.13. LPC amplitudes were larger at Pz (M = 4.88 μV, SE = 0.47) than at P3 (M = 4.58 μV, SE = 0.44; F(1, 98) = 4.56, P < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.04), P4 (M = 4.16 μV, SE = 0.43; F(1, 98) = 17.19, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.15), CP1 (M = 3.88 μV, SE = 0.47; F(1, 98) = 30.88, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.24), and CP2 (M = 3.66 μV, SE = 0.42; F(1, 98) = 63.85, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.40). Yet, the Bayes factor of the main effect Electrode position was inconclusive, BF10 = 1.52. LPC amplitudes were smaller for deceptive compared to honest responses (Figure 6A), F(1, 98) = 50.16, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.34, and also the corresponding Bayes factor suggested decisive evidence for hypothesis b, BF10 = 5.14e+67. Likewise, LPC amplitudes differed depending on the Valence of the items (Figure 6B), F(1, 98) = 8.55, P < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.08, BF10 = 20518.15. LPC amplitudes were smaller for agree compared to disagree items. The Response × Valence interaction was not significant, F(1, 98) = 2.08, P = 0.15, ηp2 = 0.02. Yet, the corresponding Bayes factor indicated evidence for the alternative hypothesis, BF10 = 50.29. The repeated measures ANOVAs including only deceptive responses revealed that LPC amplitudes were smaller for agree than for disagree items (Figure 6C), F(1, 98) = 9.29, P < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.09, BF10 = 2.55e+10 (supporting hypothesis e). The Valence effect was not significant for truthful responses, F(1, 98) = 0.89, P = 0.35, ηp2 = 0.01, BF10 = 0.92.

Graph: Fig. 6 class="chapter-para">Means and standard errors of LPC amplitudes representing (A) the Response main effect (lies vs truthful responses), (B) the Valence effect (agree vs disagree items), and (C) the Valence effect for lies (lies for agree vs lies for disagree items).

Machiavellianism and changes in the attitude ratings

Analyses on MFN, LPC and PRP amplitudes considering Machiavellianism scores revealed no significant interaction with the Machiavellianism scores of the different scales (all Ps > 0.15, BF10 between 0.22 and 0.42; hypotheses g and h were not confirmed). Likewise, no significant interaction occurred between Response and changes in the attitude rating (all Ps > 0.13, BF10 between 0.32 and 0.60). Significant results of Response and Valence remained significant when considering Machiavellianism or changes in the attitudes.

Discussion

In the present study, we found strong support that deception about attitudes relies on additional cognitive control processes used to monitor and resolve response conflicts in comparison to being honest. We replicated the following results of Johnson et al. (2008): MFN amplitudes were larger for lies indicating that lying was accompanied by stronger response conflicts than telling the truth (hypothesis a). LPC amplitudes were smaller for lies suggesting that additional cognitive resources were required for responding deceptively (hypothesis b). Lying about attitudes triggered strategic monitoring as indicated by suppressed PRP amplitudes for lies compared to truthful responses (hypothesis c). Cognitive processes also differed depending on the valence of the attitudes. Lying about positively valued items was accompanied by more intense response conflicts and was cognitively more challenging than lying about negatively valued items. MFN amplitudes were larger and LPC amplitudes suppressed for lies about items one agrees with than for lies about topics one disagrees with (hypotheses d and e). PRPs were suppressed for agree compared to disagree items revealing that ratings about positively viewed topics trigger greater strategic monitoring than ratings about negatively viewed topics (hypothesis f). In addition to replicating Johnson et al. (2008), we found that cognitive processes indicated by MFN, PRP and LPC amplitudes during lying were not moderated by Machiavellianism (hypotheses g and h not confirmed).

Even though our effect sizes were not as large as in the study by Johnson et al. (2008), they still represent clear evidence for the alternative hypothesis, as indicated by Bayes factors (cf. Jeffreys, 1961). Corresponding to the ERP results, reactions were slower and participants made more mistakes when they lied about their attitudes. The results are in line with previous reaction time and ERP studies implying that lying is accompanied by greater response inhibition and that executive control processes are needed to handle the greater cognitive demands of lies (Johnson et al. 2003, 2004, 2005, 2008; Vendemia and Buzan, 2005; Suchotzki et al. 2017). Furthermore, they reveal that MFN, LPC and PRP components are reliable indicators of the cognitive processes of deception even beyond the context of stimulus recognition.

However, bootstrap analyses revealed that, based on intra-individual patterns of the ERP components and current categorization criteria, only a small amount of people could be categorized as lying (Supplements S5 and S6) and that further research is needed to reach this goal. In accordance with conclusions from other studies, these results seem to indicate that there is, at least as of now, no Pinocchio's nose (Fischbach and Fischbach, 2005; Volbert and Banse, 2014). The goal of the present study, as in the original study by Johnson et al. (2008), was to uncover the underlying cognitive processes of deception. Both studies revealed not one but multiple ERP markers of the cognitive processes used during deception. These markers could be combined with other ERPs as well as behavioral markers, such as differences in the speed and accuracy of truthful and deceptive responses, in a deception detection algorithm (cf. Johnson , 2014). In future studies concentrating on the detection of deception, participants that only tell the truth could be compared to lying participants, making it possible to adjust the criteria for categorizing lies by considering the rate of false positives (honest participants categorized as liars) and false negatives (lying participants categorized as being honest) and finding the best way to combine ERP and behavioral markers of deception. The possibility to base such an algorithm on a variety of deception markers has the potential to combat countermeasures, since deception can be classified based on all or a subset of these markers (Johnson , 2014). It is probably very difficult to willingly manipulate all of these ERP components, especially since they occur at different, brief periods of time, index different cognitive processes, and are generated in varying brain areas (Johnson , 2014).

Machiavellianism neither modulated differences in ERP components nor in response times nor in the correctness of responses suggesting that deception was not cognitively less strenuous for individuals higher in Machiavellianism. At least in this deception setting, when rating attitudes, individuals higher in Machiavellianism do not seem to experience fewer conflicts during lying. Since persons higher in Machiavellianism deceive more frequently in their everyday life, our finding is in line and expands the result of the study by Johnson et al. (2005) that practice did neither moderate behavioral nor MFN, PRP and LPC components during deception about the memorization of words. The non-significant Machiavellianism effects may indicate that at least for lies about attitudes the components are not so much affected by the moral or ethical standards of the individual, but rather by the resources required for cognitive processing of lies (Johnson , 1986, 1988). Moreover, we found that changes in the attitude ratings (before vs after the deception task) did not result in smaller differences of MFN, PRP and LPC amplitudes during the deception task. Altogether additional analyses regarding Machiavellianism, changes in attitude ratings, as well as additional statistical tests with Bayes factors underline the stability of the ERP effects.

Limitations and future directions

As this was the first replication of Johnson et al. (2008), we conducted our study as similar as possible to the original study. We could replicate their main ERP findings with a sample from another country. The same pattern of MFN, LPC and PRP results were obtained for German as for US residents. Yet, as in the original study, participants were from a western, industrialized country. It would be interesting to analyze data from individuals beyond the western culture. Moreover, as in Johnson et al. (2008), our study sample comprised mainly students. We cannot rule out that a moderating effect of Machiavellianism occurs for a sample with more extreme values in Machiavellianism, e.g. for prisoners or a clinical sample. Furthermore, it remains for future studies to find out whether such a moderation can be found in other deception settings, for example, when lying goes along with more positive consequences than truth telling.

Conclusion

We could replicate the main ERP results of the study by Johnson et al. (2008). Lying was accompanied by larger MFN, suppressed LPC and suppressed PRP amplitudes indicating that it was accompanied by more conflicts and was cognitively more challenging than truth telling. Hence, the findings of Johnson et al. (2008) could be generalized to a German-speaking sample. Our findings indicate that it is promising to elucidate cognitive processes during deception through ERP components in non-recognition contexts. Cognitive processes, resolving response conflicts, handling the greater mental workload and applying strategic monitoring, seem to be basic when lying about attitudes, since the same patterns of LPC, MFN and PRP results were repeatedly obtained when analyzing deception of attitudes. Moreover, the patterns of these ERP components were stable for changes in attitude ratings and across individual differences in Machiavellianism.

Supplementary data

Supplementary data are available at SCAN online.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Niklas Niedermeier, Sophie Schormann, Sabine Sedlaczek, Leona Wahnschaffe and Leon Dehnhardt for their help during data collection. We thank Benjamin Cabrera for helpful advice during programming.

Conflict of interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Footnotes 1 For catch trials, participants had to indicate honestly the button assignments (see section 'Deception task'). The cognitive demands for catch trials are therefore much lower than for deceptive responses. Yet, the mean response times of the excluded participant were higher for catch trials than for trials of attitude items requiring a deceptive response indicating that the task was not performed as instructed. References Aron, A.R., Robbins, T.W., Poldrack, R.A. (2004). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 170 – 7. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.010. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 2 Azizli, N., Atkinson, B.E., Baughman, H.M., et al. (2016). Lies and crimes: dark triad, misconduct, and high-stakes deception. Personality and Individual Differences, 89, 34 – 9. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.09.034. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat 3 Beauducel, A., Brocke, B., Leue, A. (2006). Energetical bases of extraversion: effort, arousal, EEG, and performance. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 62, 212 – 23. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.12.001. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 4 Botvinick, M.M. (2007). Conflict monitoring and decision making: reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulate function. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 356 – 66. doi: 10.3758/CABN.7.4.356. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat 5 Christie, R., Geis, F.L. (1970). Studies in Machiavellianism, New York : Academic Press. doi: 10.2307/1958540. Google Scholar Google Preview OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat COPAC Crossref 6 Delorme, A., Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open sorce toolbox for analysis of single-trail EEG dynamics including independent component anlaysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 134, 9 – 21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 7 Dong, G., Wu, H., Lu, Q. (2010). Attempting to hide our real thoughts: electrophysiological evidence from truthful and deceptive responses during evaluation. Neuroscience Letters, 479, 1 – 5. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.05.014. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 8 Fazio, R.H., Pietri, E.S., Rocklage, M.D., Shook, N.J. (2015). Positive versus negative valence: asymmetries in attitude formation and generalization as fundamental individual differences. In: Olson J.M., Zanna M.P., editors. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. Burlington, NJ : Academic Press, p. 97 – 146. doi: 10.1016/bs.aesp.2014.09.002 Google Scholar Google Preview OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat COPAC Crossref 9 Fischbach, R.L., Fischbach, G.D. (2005). The brain doesn't lie. American Journal of Bioethics, 5, 54 – 5. doi: 10.1080/15265160590960979. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Gehring, W.J., Willoughby, A.R. (2004). Are all medial frontal negativities created equal? Toward a richer empirical basis for theories of action monitoring. In: Ullsperger, M., Falkenstein, M., editors. Errors, Conflicts, and the Brain: Current Opinions on Performance Monitoring, Leipzig : Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, pp. 14 – 20. Google Scholar Google Preview OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat COPAC Gershoff, A.D., Mukherjee, A., Mukhopadhyay, A. (2008). What's not to like? Preference asymmetry in the false consensus effect. Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 119 – 25. doi: 10.1086/524416. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Gibbons, H., Schnuerch, R., Wittinghofer, C., Armbrecht, A.S., Stahl, J. (2018). Detection of deception: event-related potential markers of attention and cognitive control during intentional false responses. Psychophysiology, 55, 1 – 22. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13047. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Gombos, V.A. (2006). The cognition of deception: the role of executive processes in producing lies. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 132, 197 – 214. doi: 10.3200/MONO.132.3.197-214. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Gozna, L.F., Vrij, A., Bull, R. (2001). The impact of individual differences on perceptions of lying in everyday life and in a high stake situation. Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 1203 – 16. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00219-1. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Henning, H.J., Six, B. (1977). Konstruktion einer machiavellismus-skala [construction of a Machiavellianism scale]. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 8, 185 – 98. Google Scholar OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat JASP Team (2018). JASP (Version 0.10.0.0) [Computer software]. Jasper, H.H. (1958). The ten-twenty electrode system of the international federation. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 10, 371 – 5. Google Scholar OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat Jeffreys, H. (1961). Theory of Probability, 3rd edn, Oxford : Oxford University Press. Google Scholar Google Preview OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat COPAC Johnson, R., Jr. (1986). A triarchic model of P300 amplitude. Psychophysiology, 23, 367 – 84. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1986.tb00649.x. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Johnson, R., Jr. (1988). The amplitude of the P300 component of the event-related potential: Review and synthesis. In: Ackles, P., Jennings, J.R., Coles, M.G.H., editors. Advances in Psychophysiology, Greenwich, CT : JAI Press, pp. 69 – 137. Google Scholar Google Preview OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat COPAC Johnson, R. Jr. (2014). The neural basis of deception and credibility assessment: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. In: Credibility Assessment: Scientific Research and Applications. Oxford : Elsevier, p. 217 – 300. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-394433-7.00006-3. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS Google Preview WorldCat COPAC Johnson, R., Jr., Barnhardt, J., Zhu, J. (2003). The deceptive response: effects of response conflict and strategic monitoring on the late positive component and episodic memory-related brain activity. Biological Psychology, 64, 217 – 53. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2003.07.006. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Johnson, R., Jr., Barnhardt, J., Zhu, J. (2004). The contribution of executive processes to deceptive responding. Neuropsychologia, 42, 878 – 901. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.005. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Johnson, R., Jr., Barnhardt, J., Zhu, J. (2005). Differential effects of practice on the executive processes used for truthful and deceptive responses: an event-related brain potential study. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 386 – 404. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.02.011. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Johnson, R., Jr., Henkell, H., Simon, E., Zhu, J. (2008). The self in conflict: the role of executive processes during truthful and deceptive responses about attitudes. NeuroImage, 39, 469 – 82. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.08.032. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Jones, D.N., Paulhus, D.L. (2014). Introducing the short dark triad (SD3): a brief measure of dark personality traits. Assessment, 21, 28 – 41. doi: 10.1177/1073191113514105. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Kashy, D.A., DePaulo, B.M. (1996). Who lies? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1037 – 51. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.5.1037. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Kireev, M.V., Pakhomov, S.V., Medvedev, S.V. (2008). Cerebral mechanisms of error detection during deceptive responses in the normal state and under the influence of alcohol. Human Physiology, 34, 141 – 9. doi: 10.1134/S0362119708020023. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Lee, M.D., Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2013). Bayesian Cognitive Modeling: A Practical Course, New York, NY : C. U. Press. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS Google Preview WorldCat COPAC Leue, A., Beauducel, A. (2019). A meta-analysis of the P3 amplitude in tasks requiring deception in legal and social contexts. Brain and Cognition, 135, 103564. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2019.05.002. Google Scholar OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat Crossref Leue, A., Lange, S., Beauducel, A. (2012). "Have you ever seen this face?"—individual differences and event-related potentials during deception. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 1 – 13. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00570. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Meek, S.W., Phillips, M.C., Boswell, C.P., Vendemia, J.M.C. (2013). Deception and the misinformation effect: an event-related potential study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 87, 81 – 7. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.11.004. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Meijer, E.H., Smulders, F.T.Y., Merckelbach, H.L.G.J., Wolf, A.G. (2007). The P300 is sensitive to concealed face recognition. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 66, 231 – 37. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.08.001. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Nieuwenhuis, S., Holroyd, C.B., Mol, N., Coles, M.G.H. (2004). Reinforcement-related brain potentials from medial frontal cortex: origins and functional significance. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 28, 441 – 8. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.05.003. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Olson, J., Rosenfeld, J.P., Kim, T., Perrault, E. (2018). The effect of countermeasures against the reaction time based concealed information test on the P300 index of knowledge recognition: a combined RT and P300-based test. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 134, 9 – 14. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2018.10.003. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Open Science Collaboration (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science, 349, 943-aac4716-8. doi: 10.1126/science.aac4716. OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat Crossref Palmer, B., Nasman, V.T., Wilson, G.F. (1994). Task decision difficulty: effects on ERPs in a same-different letter classification task. Biological Psychology, 38, 199 – 214. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(94)90039-6. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Panasiti, M.S., Pavone, E.F., Mancini, A., Merla, A., Grisoni, L., Aglioti, S.M. (2014). The motor cost of telling lies: electrocortical signatures and personality foundations of spontaneous deception. Social Neuroscience, 9, 573 – 89. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2014.934394. Google Scholar PubMed OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat Pfister, R., Foerster, A., Kunde, W. (2014). Pants on fire: the electrophysiological signature of telling a lie. Social Neuroscience, 9, 562 – 72. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2014.934392. Google Scholar PubMed OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118, 2128 – 48. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(95)05130-9 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Rosenfeld, J.P., Angell, A., Johnson, M., Qian, J.-H. (1991). An ERP-based, control-question lie detector analog: algorithms for discriminating effects within individuals' average waveforms. Psychophysiology, 28, 319 – 35. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1991.tb02202.x. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Rosenfeld, J.P., Hu, X., Labkovsky, E., Meixner, J., Winograd, M.R. (2013). Review of recent studies and issues regarding the P300-based complex trial protocol for detection of concealed information. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 90, 118 – 34. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.08.012. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Scheuble, V., Beauducel, A. (2020). Individual differences in ERPs during deception: observing vs. demonstrating behavior leading to a small social conflict. Biological Psychology, 150, 1 – 11. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2019.107830. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Sip, K.E., Roepstorff, A., McGregor, W., Frith, C.D. (2008). Detecting deception: the scope and limits. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 48 – 53. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.11.008. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Suchotzki, K., Crombez, G., Smulders, F.T.Y., Meijer, E., Verschuere, B. (2015). The cognitive mechanisms underlying deception: an event-related potential study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 95, 395 – 405. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.01.010. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Suchotzki, K., Verschuere, B., Van Bockstaele, B., Ben-Shakhar, G., Crombez, G. (2017). Lying takes time: a meta-analysis on reaction time measures of deception. Psychological Bulletin, 143, 428 – 53. doi: 10.1037/bul0000087. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Tu, S., Li, H., Jou, J., Zhang, Q., Wang, T., Yu, C., et al. (2009). An event-related potential study of deception to self preferences. Brain Research, 1247, 142 – 8. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.09.090. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Vendemia, J.M.C., Buzan, R.F. (2005). Neural mechanisms of deception and response congruity in a visual two-stimulus paradigm with motor response. Polygraph, 34, 24 – 46. Google Scholar OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat Volbert, R., Banse, R. (2014). Deception detection: how can psychological research assist legal practice? European Psychologist, 19, 159 – 61. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000209. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Wissing, B.G., Reinhard, M.A. (2019). The dark triad and deception perceptions. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1 – 7. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01811. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Yong, E. (2012). Bad copy. Nature, 485, 298 – 300. doi: 10.1038/485298a. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat

By V Scheuble and A Beauducel

Reported by Author; Author

Correspondence should be addressed to Vera Scheuble, Institute of Psychology, University of Bonn, Kaiser-Karl-Ring 9, 53111 Bonn, Germany

Titel:
Cognitive processes during deception about attitudes revisited: a replication study
Autor/in / Beteiligte Person: Scheuble, Vera ; Beauducel, André
Link:
Zeitschrift: Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, Jg. 15 (2020-01-24), Heft 8
Veröffentlichung: 2020
Medientyp: unknown
ISSN: 1749-5024 (print)
Schlagwort:
  • Adult
  • Male
  • Deception
  • Adolescent
  • Cognitive Neuroscience
  • media_common.quotation_subject
  • AcademicSubjects/SCI01880
  • MFN
  • Experimental and Cognitive Psychology
  • Original Manuscript
  • 050105 experimental psychology
  • 03 medical and health sciences
  • Young Adult
  • 0302 clinical medicine
  • Cognition
  • Replication (statistics)
  • Humans
  • Machiavellianism
  • 0501 psychology and cognitive sciences
  • Control (linguistics)
  • Evoked Potentials
  • media_common
  • attitudes
  • 05 social sciences
  • Brain
  • General Medicine
  • LPC
  • Attitude
  • Female
  • Psychology
  • Lying
  • 030217 neurology & neurosurgery
  • Cognitive psychology
Sonstiges:
  • Nachgewiesen in: OpenAIRE
  • Rights: OPEN

Klicken Sie ein Format an und speichern Sie dann die Daten oder geben Sie eine Empfänger-Adresse ein und lassen Sie sich per Email zusenden.

oder
oder

Wählen Sie das für Sie passende Zitationsformat und kopieren Sie es dann in die Zwischenablage, lassen es sich per Mail zusenden oder speichern es als PDF-Datei.

oder
oder

Bitte prüfen Sie, ob die Zitation formal korrekt ist, bevor Sie sie in einer Arbeit verwenden. Benutzen Sie gegebenenfalls den "Exportieren"-Dialog, wenn Sie ein Literaturverwaltungsprogramm verwenden und die Zitat-Angaben selbst formatieren wollen.

xs 0 - 576
sm 576 - 768
md 768 - 992
lg 992 - 1200
xl 1200 - 1366
xxl 1366 -