Zum Hauptinhalt springen

What Do Patients Value as Incentives for Participation in Clinical Trials? A Pilot Discrete Choice Experiment

Vellinga, Akke ; Devine, Colum ; et al.
In: Research Ethics, Jg. 16 (2020), Heft 1-2, S. 12
Online academicJournal

What do patients value as incentives for participation in clinical trials? A pilot discrete choice experiment 

Incentivising has shown to improve participation in clinical trials. However, ethical concerns suggest that incentives may be coercive, obscure trial risks and encourage individuals to enrol in clinical trials for the wrong reasons. The aim of our study was to develop and pilot a discrete choice experiment (DCE) to explore and identify preferences for incentives. A DCE was designed by including following attributes (and levels) of incentives: value, method, and time involvement. To account for trial benefit and risk, each was included as an attribute with levels low, medium and high. For testing purposes, the DCE was administrated using SurveyMonkey in a population of third level students. A total of 245 students, representative of the general student population, participated in the online DCE. The results provide a template to assess and explore the use of different incentive methods in clinical trials. The template can be used in its current format or adapted to particular scenarios. This pilot study provides a feasible methodology to explore the use of incentives for participation in clinical trials and can be adapted to specific trial requirements to provide information for ethical applications or identify the most favourable incentive for participation in clinical trials.

Keywords: Randomised controlled trial; incentive; discrete choice experiment

Introduction/background

According to the 1947 Nuremberg Code, no persuasion or pressure of any kind should be put on clinical trial participants ([19]). Incentives in research are defined to be payments, recompense and rewards ([5]). In this context, incentives for patients may be seen as coercive, or as exerting undue influence on potential participants' decisions about whether to take part in research ([22]; [23]). Whereas incentives for physicians are generally well accepted, to compensate for increased workload and/or time, incentives to encourage patients to participate in health research or clinical trials are often disfavoured ([25]).

Incentivising the consent procedure has shown to improve recall in participants, particularly in relation to potential serious side effects ([10]). The research participants' motivation is superior to the influence of payments and financial rewards do not distort the participants' behaviour or blind them to the risks involved with research ([1]). A study using lottery tickets as an incentive to return a mail questionnaire had an opposing effect as the intrinsic motivation of the respondents was already high ([24]). Other use of incentives to improve questionnaire return was tested by the inclusion of a dollar note or a lottery ticket with the dollar note and these inducements had a slightly higher impact but either better than no incentive at all ([26]). Incentives can also improve the generalisability reflected in a study in which the inclusion of a scratch lottery tickets improved survey response and representativeness through the participation of more respondents who had lower education ([20]). The use of incentives in questionnaire surveys is an accepted practice and a meta-analysis concluded that researchers should consider including small amounts of money with mailed questionnaires rather than give no incentive at all ([9]).

Incentives are however more questionable when participants find themselves in a dependency relationship with the researcher (or physician), where the risks are particularly high, where the research is degrading, where the participant will only consent if the incentive is relatively large because the participant's aversion to the study is strong and where the aversion is a principled one ([11]). It is therefore important to be transparent and describe any system of payment or reimbursement in detail ([8]).

The use of financial incentives in clinical trials is not well studied and generally less accepted. Hospital- or community-based trials need to take extra care to avoid incentives that may be coercive or unduly influence research participants ([4]). Incentivising patients may include payment for time to participate (for its potential motivational benefits), small gifts, payment for incidental expenses, cash or voucher incentives for participation and retention. A recent study reported current practice and future priorities to improve patient recruitment and retention in clinical trials ([5]) and concludes that the scope for testing incentives through formal experimental methods may be limited by ethical and equity considerations. Concern was more often expressed when payment was described as 'substantial' compared to 'tokens' and gift cards (vouchers) and non-monetary gifts were not considered to influence participation ([17]). Interestingly however, a larger amount offered as an incentive was not found to be more efficacious in motivating a subject to participate ([25]). It is important to acknowledge that incentives work both ways: patients feel acknowledged for their participation and are more encouraged to invite eligible patients into a trial ([21]).

Ethical committees or institutional research board members often struggle with concepts of reimbursement and incentivisation. While members may agree with reimbursement or compensation for time and inconvenience, they may not agree with payment for participation or compensation for risk ([17]). Undue influence as well as coercion were considered to be violated with the offer of payment even though the literature reports payment of participants highly effective and securing participation ([3]). This paper is of particular interest as it considers if and how monetary payments influences subjects' participation and risk evaluation. This 'willingness to pay' analysis presented three imaginary studies varying the levels of risk and monetary payment and 207 students were asked to answer a number of questions on their study–risk combination. This paper concludes that (higher levels of) monetary payment increased students' participation in research irrespective of the risk of the study. This study was limited by proposing studies for healthy volunteers, which may have affected the lack of attention to risk.

Difficulties with research to determine the use of incentives for participation include working with 'example' studies to convey different levels of risk and the need for large samples. Taking some of the limitations into account, we explored alternative options to study choice and gain a better understanding of the elements involved in choice-making ([14]). Discrete choice experiments (DCE) are widely used in health economics to assess preferences ([6]). DCEs have gained attention with the increasing interest in public and patient preferences to inform clinical and policy decision-making. One advantage of a DCE is that it does not need to work with scenarios but can allow each participant to interpret the study question in their own context.

DCEs can be used to propose choice sets with hypothetical options in relation to incentives introduced in the context of clinical trials. DCE can disentangle stated preferences, or what an individual says they would do, from observed preferences, what the individuals actually does, and compare these to current practice and standards in clinical trials and health research in general. DCEs involve the generation and analysis of choice data in the context of hypothetical scenarios ([18]). The first step of a DCE is to select the attributes of interest and their levels. The second step is to combine attributes into choice sets. The combination relies on experimental plan theory as a full factorial design generally implies proposing too many choices to respondents.

Our aim was to develop and pilot a discrete choice experiment to explore and measure the use of incentives to recruit patients to clinical trials.

Methodology

Development of the DCE

A DCE asks individuals to state their preference of hypothetical alternatives, in this case incentives. Each alternative is described by its attributes or characteristics and responses are used to infer the value placed on each attribute. In stating a preference the individual is assumed to choose the alternative that yields the highest individual benefit (utility) ([12]). The choice made by the individual allows the influence of the underlying characteristics on the individual's decision to be estimated. Comparing all the choices shows how individuals trade-off one attribute for another.

Following best practice in designing DCEs, a qualitative approach was taken to identify attributes and levels in a two-step process. First, international literature on incentives in clinical trials was reviewed to identify all the relevant attributes. For each attribute, potential levels were recorded. This literature review was then used to inform an expert group including experts in trial methodology, health economics, epidemiology, social marketing and statistics. The expert group defined the final research question and identified the most relevant and attainable attributes (Table 1).

Graph

Table 1. DCE attributes and levels.

AttributeExplanationLevels
ValueThe monetary value paid to you to take part in the trial• €0*• €30• €60
MethodThe form in which this payment is made• Cash*• Gift• Voucher
Time involvementThe time commitment that you will need to make to take part in the trial• A single one-hour session*• 30 minutes per day for 3 weeks• 30 minutes per week for 3 months
Trial benefitsThe possible advantages to you of taking part in the trial, e.g. access to a new treatment which would not otherwise be available to you• Low• Medium• High
Trial risksThe possible disadvantages to you of taking part in the trial, e.g. the chance that the intervention will have harmful side effects• Low• Medium• High

1 Denotes reference group.

The decision was made to avoid scenarios to confer risk, as interpretation of scenarios is dependent on the individuals' circumstances (for instance age or gender). A more generalisable approach was taken by introducing the DCE with an introduction to randomised trials. The introduction then includes, 'for this example, you have been asked to participate in a trial comparing two interventions to improve your health'. Full instructions and an example scenario are provided in Figure 1. To convey a level of risk and benefit, each scenario included these as attributes with three levels (low, medium, high). The attributes 'benefits' and 'risks' are inherent to each trial, related for instance to the therapeutic effect or adverse effect of an intervention, and cannot be modified by researchers for the purpose of increasing recruitment. However, risk and benefit of the trial influences the decision-making of any prospective participant, so excluding them from the list of attributes would be likely to increase random or unexplainable utility in data analysis ([7]; [13]). Therefore, risk and benefit were included in the DCE as attributes to allow a correction for these confounders in the final analysis.

Graph: Figure 1. DCE instructions and example of choice set.

To answer the question 'what preferences encourage people to participate in clinical trials?' a small-scale study was proposed to test each element of the DCE in a particular population. As each population allows different approaches, the focus of the presented DCE was a student population, allowing for online DCEs and electronic invitations through social media platforms (Facebook, WhatsApp, university mailing list).

Experimental design

The final DCE included the following attributes: value, method, time involvement, trial risk, trial benefit (Table 1). Based on these attributes and levels, a total of 243 (35, 5 attributes with 3 levels) possible options, with a total of 29,403 possible choice sets (i.e. the full factorial design) were generated. We used IBM SPSS version 23.0 to generate the choice sets based on orthogonal arrays. Orthogonal arrays assume that attributes are statistically independent and support the balance of the levels in the final DCE. After discussion, a total of nine scenarios or choice sets with minimal overlap were selected (fractional factorial design) to minimise cognitive fatigue ([2]).

The surveys were transferred to SurveyMonkey as images and the individual's choice (A or B) was recorded electronically. Additional demographic information (age, gender, discipline, educational background, in receipt of study funding, and previous experience with health research) was also collected. Survey links were distributed through the university's student email system and by sharing the link on Facebook and WhatsApp groups. The survey was live for a seven-day period in respect of the amount of student communication (28 September to 5 October 2017). Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the Social Research Ethics Committee at the University College Cork.

Analysis

The underlying principle of DCEs is based on the consumer theory of demand, which states that when an individual is faced with different choices, he/she will choose the alternative that provides the highest utility ('happiness') ([15]). The random utility theory (RUT) is therefore used to analyse and explain choices made in DCEs ([16]). The RUT divides utility, a measure of preference, into a random component, due to unobserved attributes or variation between and within individuals or error, and a systematic component. The systematic component is a function of the attributes and its levels and allocates an amount of the variation that can be explained by each attribute.

Each scenario is judged as a bundle of characteristics (attributes/level) to be compared with an alternative scenario. No scenario will present the ideal options, but we assume that the individual will choose the choice set that will provide them with the highest benefit (utility). With each choice set having a different combination, the effect of each attribute level on the chance of the choice set being chosen can be estimated with a random effects logistic regression analysis accounting for multiple answers by each individuals (STATA v13). The estimated coefficients represent the preference of each attribute level influencing the choice, compared to the lowest (reference) attribute level. This approach limits the number of comparisons and results in two coefficients for each attribute.

A preference heterogeneity analysis was also performed to investigate the influence of selected demographic characteristics. The variability in preferences is investigated by a comparison of the marginal effect of each personal characteristic on the sample level preferences (for instance, what is the marginal effect of studying science on average preference for an incentive). However, this approach results in a large number of comparisons. The use of a reference category as well as restriction in the number of interactions tested, was therefore applied.

  • Gender (reference: Male)
  • Discipline (reference: College of Arts, Social sciences and Celtic studies)
  • Previous experience in clinical studies (reference: No)
  • In receipt of a study grant (reference: No)
Results

All the questions were answered by 245 students, 159 (64.9%) of whom were female (Table 2). Their mean age was 22, ranging between 17 and 55.

Graph

Table 2. Demographic overview of the student population.

N%
Total245
Female15964.9
Male8534.7
Unknown10.4
Discipline
College of Arts, Social Sciences and Celtic Studies5321.6
College of Science, Medicine and Nursing12350.2
College of Business, Public Policy and Law3313.5
College of Engineering and Informatics2610.6
Level of study
Undergraduate21186.1
Postgraduate3413.9
Recipient of study grant
No8534.7
Yes16065.3
Previous experience in clinical studies
No20684.1
Yes3915.9

Preference analysis

Students prefer an incentive with a higher value and compared to no value, students are 1.9 times more likely to opt for €30 and 5.6 times more likely to choose €60 (Table 3). Students prefer cash compared to vouchers (Odds ratio 0.7) or gifts (odds ratio 0.6). Students prefer a one-off time involvement compared to a 30 minutes per week for 3 months. A shorter duration of time involvement, i.e. daily for 3 weeks versus weekly for 3 months, is also preferred by students.

Graph

Table 3. Preference analysis of the attributes of incentives.

Odds ratio95% Confidence interval
Value
€0Reference
€301.91.5–2.4
€605.64.5–6.9
Method
CashReference
Voucher0.70.5–0.8
Gift0.60.4–0.7
Time involvement
30 minutes/week for 3 monthsReference
Single one-hour session2.62.1–3.2
30 minutes/day for 3 weeks1.81.4–2.2

None of the included demographic variables influence the choice of students and no differences between colleges can be observed in relation to value or method of incentive. Engineering and informatics students show a slight preference for a single one-hour session. Level of study, recipients of study grants or previous experiences in clinical studies is not associated with a change in preferences.

Participation in the DCE

A total of 200 out of 245 participants are included in this pilot study; 45 students were excluded as they did not complete the DCE. The largest drop off happened after obtaining the participant information (29); once participants started the survey, most of them finished it (7 drop out after the first scenario, the remaining 9 subsequently).

Discussion

The application of a DCE to assess preferences in incentives has shown to be successful. Despite being a small-scale pilot study, this modified DCE provides insights into how people choose incentives in relation to participation in clinical trials.

The DCE was developed to test, in its broadest application, the variation in the use of incentives, depending on the benefits and risks of the study. For this reason, the DCE did not include a scenario, as previous experiences would be determining the interpretation of risk and benefit. However, the use of scenarios with or without including specific benefits and risks may improve the application of understanding preferences in particular populations or for particular studies. This pilot study provides a template for use in specific studies or trials, or broader implementation to determine preferences.

It is the first time a DCE methodology is applied in this context to explore the value of incentives for participation in a clinical trial. In this study setting the risk and benefit at different levels allows participants' own personal interpretation of risk and benefit. Other attributes such as levels of monetary incentives, type of incentives and time commitment were pre-set. These attributes as well as their levels could be changed and adapted to other situations to determine preferences for incentives. Limiting the number of comparisons by predetermining the variables of interest as well as setting up models based on pre-specified hypothesis, will help the interpretation and application of a DCE.

In conclusion, we provide a template to explore and determine preferences for incentives for recruitment of participant to clinical trials. The presented methodology will allow researcher to support ethical applications as well as identify the most appropriate incentives for a proposed trial.

We would like to thank Dr Darren Dahly, statistician at the HRB Clinical Research Facility Cork and School of Public Health, University College Cork for his help as member of the expert team.

References 1 Ballantyne A (2008) Benefits to research subjects in international trials: do they reduce exploitation or increase undue inducement? Developing World Bioethics8(3): 178–191. 2 Bekker-Grob EWd, Ryan M, Gerard K (2012) Discrete choice experiments in health economics: a review of the literature. Health Economics21(2): 145–172. 3 Bentley JP, Thacker PG (2004) The influence of risk and monetary payment on the research participation decision making process. Journal of Medical Ethics30(3): 293–298. 4 Bernstein SL, Feldman J (2015) Incentives to participate in clinical trials: practical and ethical considerations. The American Journal of Emergency Medicine33(9): 1197–1200. 5 Bower P, Brueton V, Gamble C, et al. (2014) Interventions to improve recruitment and retention in clinical trials: a survey and workshop to assess current practice and future priorities. Trials15: 399. 6 Clark MD, Determann D, Petrou S, et al. (2014) Discrete choice experiments in health economics: a review of the literature. Pharmacoeconomics32(9): 883–902. 7 Ding EL, Powe NR, Manson JE, et al. (2007) Sex differences in perceived risks, distrust, and willingness to participate in clinical trials: a randomized study of cardiovascular prevention trials. Archives of Internal Medicine167(9): 905–912. 8 Draper H, Wilson S, Flanagan S, et al. (2009) Offering payments, reimbursement and incentives to patients and family doctors to encourage participation in research. Family Practice26(3): 231–238. 9 Edwards P, Cooper R, Roberts I, et al. (2005) Meta-analysis of randomised trials of monetary incentives and response to mailed questionnaires. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health59(11): 987–999. Festinger DS, Marlowe DB, Croft JR, et al. (2009) Monetary incentives improve recall of research consent information: it pays to remember. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology17(2): 99–104. Grant RW, Sugarman J (2004) Ethics in human subjects research: do incentives matter? The Journal of Medicine and Philosophy29(6): 717–738. Hall J, Viney R, Haas M, et al. (2004) Using stated preference discrete choice modeling to evaluate health care programs. Journal of Business Research57(9): 1026–1032. Halpern SD, Karlawish JH, Casarett D, et al. (2004) Empirical assessment of whether moderate payments are undue or unjust inducements for participation in clinical trials. Archives of Internal Medicine164(7): 801–803. Hensher D, Rose J, Greene W (2005) Applied Choice Analysis: A Primer. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Lancaster KJ (1966) A new approach to consumer theory. Journal of Political Economy74: 132. Lancsar E, Louviere J (2008) Conducting discrete choice experiments to inform healthcare decision making: a user's guide. Pharmacoeconomics26(8): 661–677. Largent EA, Grady C, Miller FG, et al. (2012) Money, coercion, and undue inducement: attitudes about payments to research participants. IRB34(1): 1–8. Louviere JJ, Lancsar E (2009) Choice experiments in health: the good, the bad, the ugly and toward a brighter future. Health Economics, Policy, and Law4(Pt 4): 527–546. Moreno JD, Schmidt U, Joffe S (2017) The nuremberg code 70 years later. JAMA318(9): 795–796. Olsen F, Abelsen B, Olsen JA (2012) Improving response rate and quality of survey data with a scratch lottery ticket incentive. BMC Medical Research Methodology12: 52. Rendell JM, Merritt RK, Geddes J (2007) Incentives and disincentives to participation by clinicians in randomised controlled trials. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews18(2): MR00021. Singer E, Bossarte RM (2006) Incentives for survey participation: when are they "Coercive"? American Journal of Preventive Medicine31(5): 411–418. Singer E, Couper MP (2008) Do incentives exert undue influence on survey participation? Experimental evidence. Journal of Empirical Research on Human Research Ethics: JERHRE3(3): 49–56. Wenemark M, Vernby A, Norberg AL (2010) Can incentives undermine intrinsic motivation to participate in epidemiologic surveys? European Journal of Epidemiology25(4): 231–235. Wertheimer A, Miller FG (2008) Payment for research participation: a coercive offer? Journal of Medical Ethics34(5): 389–392. Whiteman MK, Langenberg P, Kjerulff K, et al. (2003) A randomized trial of incentives to improve response rates to a mailed women's health questionnaire. Journal of Women's Health (Larchmt)12(8): 821–828. Footnotes Conflict of Interests None reported. Funding All articles in Research Ethics are published as open access. There are no submission charges and no Article Processing Charges as these are fully funded by institutions through Knowledge Unlatched, resulting in no direct charge to authors. For more information about Knowledge Unlatched please see here: http://www.knowledgeunlatched.org ORCID iD Akke Vellinga Graph https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6583-4300

By Akke Vellinga; Colum Devine; Min Yun Ho; Colin Clarke; Patrick Leahy; Jane Bourke; Declan Devane; Sinead Duane and Patricia Kearney

Reported by Author; Author; Author; Author; Author; Author; Author; Author; Author

Titel:
What Do Patients Value as Incentives for Participation in Clinical Trials? A Pilot Discrete Choice Experiment
Autor/in / Beteiligte Person: Vellinga, Akke ; Devine, Colum ; Ho, Min Yun ; Clarke, Colin ; Leahy, Patrick ; Bourke, Jane ; Devane, Declan ; Duane, Sinead ; Kearney, Patricia
Link:
Zeitschrift: Research Ethics, Jg. 16 (2020), Heft 1-2, S. 12
Veröffentlichung: 2020
Medientyp: academicJournal
ISSN: 1747-0161 (print)
DOI: 10.1177/1747016119898669
Schlagwort:
  • Descriptors: Patients Value Judgment Incentives Randomized Controlled Trials Participation Ethics Experiments Preferences Recruitment Time Management Risk Assessment College Students Foreign Countries
  • Geographic Terms: Ireland
Sonstiges:
  • Nachgewiesen in: ERIC
  • Sprachen: English
  • Language: English
  • Peer Reviewed: Y
  • Page Count: 12
  • Document Type: Journal Articles ; Reports - Research
  • Education Level: Higher Education ; Postsecondary Education
  • Abstractor: As Provided
  • Entry Date: 2020

Klicken Sie ein Format an und speichern Sie dann die Daten oder geben Sie eine Empfänger-Adresse ein und lassen Sie sich per Email zusenden.

oder
oder

Wählen Sie das für Sie passende Zitationsformat und kopieren Sie es dann in die Zwischenablage, lassen es sich per Mail zusenden oder speichern es als PDF-Datei.

oder
oder

Bitte prüfen Sie, ob die Zitation formal korrekt ist, bevor Sie sie in einer Arbeit verwenden. Benutzen Sie gegebenenfalls den "Exportieren"-Dialog, wenn Sie ein Literaturverwaltungsprogramm verwenden und die Zitat-Angaben selbst formatieren wollen.

xs 0 - 576
sm 576 - 768
md 768 - 992
lg 992 - 1200
xl 1200 - 1366
xxl 1366 -